From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Fredrik Fasting Torgersen in the center of a police lineup.

A police lineup (in American English) or identity parade (in British English) is a process by which a crime victim or witness's putative identification of a suspect is confirmed to a level that can count as evidence at trial.

The suspect, along with several "fillers" or "foils"—people of similar height, build, and complexion who may be prisoners, actors, police officers, or volunteers—stand side-by-side, both facing and in profile. There is crucial information that should be conveyed to the eyewitness prior to viewing the lineup. It is necessary to inform the eyewitness that it is possible the perpetrator is not present in the lineup. [1] The eyewitness should also be told that they do not have to choose one of the people from the lineup. Including these details has shown to result in fewer misidentifications. [2] The lineup sometimes takes place in a room for the purpose, one which may feature a one-way mirror to allow a witness to remain anonymous, and may include markings on the wall to aid identifying the person's height.

For evidence from a lineup to be admissible in court, the lineup itself must be conducted fairly. The police may not say or do anything that persuades the witness to identify the suspect that they prefer. This includes loading the lineup with people who look very dissimilar to the suspect. [3]

Alternatives

The three main forms of police lineups are photographs of suspects, videos, or the original form of physically present lineups. While photos and videos are often more practical and convenient, lineups where suspects are physically present have been shown to improve identification. [4]

Photographs of the suspect and fillers can be shown to the identifier in what is called a "photo-lineup", or a "six pack". [5] If the victim or witness successfully identifies the suspect from among the fillers, the identification is considered valid. There is some research into using other methods of photo-lineup that involve the witness sequentially viewing photographs rather than simultaneously. [6]

The sequential method is considered more accurate because it prevents the witness from looking at all the suspects and merely selecting the person that most resembles the guilty person.

A "show-up" is another alternative, in which a suspect is individually shown to a witness. [7]

Many UK police forces use Video Identification Parade Electronic Recording (VIPER), a digital system wherein witnesses view video recordings of suspects and unrelated volunteers. [8]

Sequential lineup

A sequential lineup is one of the two methods used for eyewitnesses to identify criminals in police station. In a standard sequential lineup, the suspects or their photos are presented one at a time to the witnesses only once. Witnesses make decisions about each individual suspect before the next one is shown and they do not know the total number of suspects.

History

Although it is hard to pinpoint exactly when sequential lineups were first studied, the knowledge that simultaneous lineups often failed and convicted an innocent person has been common knowledge for many years. The advance of the popularity of sequential lineups can be traced to the Innocence Project and Gary Wells. Wells has many studies that show that sequential lineups lead to fewer wrongful convictions. The early studies of sequential lineups found that there was a significant difference in the wrongful conviction of innocent persons. Since these early studies there has been a push to increase the accuracy of eyewitness memory even more.

One way this is accomplished is by having not just sequential lineups, but also double-blind sequential lineups. A double-blind sequential lineup is conducted by making sure that neither the witness nor the person conducting the lineup has any idea who the suspect is. This eliminates any bias the person conducting the lineup may have. The research for double-blind studies has shown that "now we have proof from the field that witnesses who view double-blind sequential lineups are just as likely to pick the suspect, and perhaps more importantly, less likely to make a misidentification by picking a filler in the lineup." [9]

The study of sequential lineups is far from being finished and there is still much left to prove. The New York Times reported that Wells will continue to "examine the data gathered to gauge the level of certainty of witnesses and the effect of factors like cross-racial identification on accuracy." [10]

Sequential lineup laps

A sequential lineup lap is showing the suspects repeatedly after the first round while in a standard sequential lineup eyewitnesses can only view them once. The thinking is that viewing the suspects again can increase the accuracy of identification since the eyewitnesses will be more certain about their answer.

Research found that viewing the suspects once more has a large influence on witnesses' performance. Many witnesses moved from no-choice to choice, some changed answers and their confidence went up. Both correct identification rate and mistaken rate increased in a sequential lineup lap when the suspect was present; the error rate increased only when the suspect was absent. [11]

US law

While many states agree that sequential lineups can reduce wrongful convictions, they also notice that sequential lineups lead to more of a chance that the guilty would be overlooked and not convicted of their crime. Because of this many states do not want to implement a law that mandates sequential lineups. These states accept their benefits but do not want to rule out other types of lineup. There are different feelings about the advantages and disadvantages of sequential lineups. Gronlund, Carlson, Dailey, and Goodsell state one of the disadvantages: "Sequential lineups do not enhance accuracy but rather make eyewitnesses more conservative in their willingness to choose. Although this is desirable when the police have an innocent suspect, it is problematic if the police have a guilty one." [12] But Lindsay, Mansour, Beaudry, Leach, and Bertrand show one of the advantages of sequential lineups estimating that with them between 570 and 1425 innocent people would not be wrongfully convicted that would be with simultaneous lineups. [13] According to the Innocence Project website, many states and law enforcement agencies have started to implement the tools that would be necessary to run double-blind sequential lineups but have yet to fully embrace them. [10]

Studies

Cutler and Penrod (1988) [14]

Brain L. Cutler and Steven D. Penrod conducted this study in 1988 to examine multiple variables' influence on eyewitnesses' accuracy during a lineup. The participants were first given a videotaped store robbery and a questionnaire, then asked to identify the robber in a photo lineup. They were given different videotapes, different lineups, and different instructions. There were 175 participants, all undergraduate college students.

The results were:

  • Correct identification rate: 80% for sequential lineups and 76% for simultaneous lineups (total).
    • 78% for sequential lineups and 80% for simultaneous lineups when cues were strong.
    • 84% for sequential lineups and 58% for simultaneous lineups when cues were weak.

Mistaken rate in target-absent condition: 19% for sequential lineups and 39% for sequential lineups.

In this study, the correct identification rates were very much higher across all situations than normal. They also did not find a significant enough difference in correct identification rate between simultaneous and sequential lineups when the target was present. Most studies found that the correct identification rate is higher for simultaneous lineups.

Steblay, Dysart, and Wells (2011) [11]

In 2011, Steblay, Dysart, and Wells attempted to answer a debate that has been around since the concept of sequential lineups. Are they superior to simultaneous lineups? In an effort to reproduce the results found in previous studies done on sequential lineups, Steblay, Dysart, and Wells took and combined results from 72 tests from 23 different labs from across the world including Canada, the United Kingdom, the United States, Germany, and South Africa. These results included data from 13,143 people who participated as witness in the studies. In this study they found very similar results to previous studies that have been conducted. They found that sequential lineups are less likely to identify any type (whether guilty or not guilty) of a suspect than simultaneous lineups, but that when a suspect was identified he/she was more likely to be guilty using this method than a simultaneous lineup.

They found that there is an 8% difference in suspect identification between sequential and simultaneous lineups, favoring simultaneous lineups; meaning that simultaneous lineups are more likely overall to identify the guilty suspect. This finding has decreased since 2001 where there was a 15% difference in favor of simultaneous lineups. [15] They also replicated the findings that there is about a 22% difference between sequential and simultaneous lineups regarding errors in suspect identification; meaning that sequential lineups are less likely to identify the wrong suspect.

Fraud

The police can falsify the results of a lineup by giving hints to the witness. For example, they may let the witness "accidentally" see their preferred suspect in circumstances indicating criminality (e.g., in handcuffs) before the lineup. [16] This is sometimes called an "Oklahoma showup" and was claimed to have been used in the Caryl Chessman case. [17]

Error rate

Limitations of technology

The use of DNA evidence has allowed for greater accuracy in choosing a suspect. It is evident that misidentification is not uncommon with police lineups. In a study published by the Association for Psychological Science, scientists discovered that in a group of 349 people that had been exonerated with DNA evidence, 258 of these people (roughly 3 out of every 4) were involved in mistaken eyewitness identification. [18]

Familiarity

Mere exposure to a face can contribute to the illusion that an innocent person is the guilty party. For example, a witness might identify a receptionist as the guilty suspect simply because they had met briefly before, misattributing the familiarity to seeing the individual committing the crime. This is a source-monitoring error, where the familiarity is misattributed and unconsciously transferred to an innocent bystander. [19] [20] See also Familiarity heuristic.

Own-group recognition bias

Witnesses are more likely to correctly identify the faces of those with whom they share common features, such as race, age, and gender. [21] [22] [23] See also cross-race effect, in-group favoritism, sex differences in eyewitness memory.

Framing bias

Subtle framing characteristics influence a witness' response to a question. For example, if a police officer asks which of the individuals in a lineup committed the crime, the wording of the question implies that one of the individuals is guilty, in a manner analogous to leading questions in court testimony. This suggestion increases the likelihood that the witness will pick someone from the lineup without positive recognition. [24]

Additionally, the overwhelming majority of witnesses will identify a suspect from a lineup even if the actual perpetrator is not included in the lineup, often depending on how the instructions for choosing a suspect are presented. [25] [26] See also Framing effect (psychology).

Feedback

Either confirming or refuting feedback to has been shown to distort witnesses' reported perception of a suspect. Providing feedback to a witness after identifying a suspect can change the way they recall the quality and clarity of their perception of the incident, the speed and certainty of their identification, and other factors, even when witnesses believed the feedback had not influenced their report. [25] [24] During questioning or viewing pictures in a lineup, it was found that an eyewitness made a tentative judgement that a certain picture might be the guilty suspect, to which an officer administering the lineup answered, "okay." However, upon returning to that picture months later at trial, the witness expressed no doubt that the previously hypothesized picture represented the suspect. [25]

Fillers

According to a 2021 study, optimal lineups have fillers who are similar to the description of the perpetrator of a crime, but who are otherwise dissimilar to the suspect. [27] When fillers are highly similar to the suspect, it increases the chances that witnesses cannot make a possible identification. [27] When fillers are highly dissimilar to the suspect, it increases the chances that witnesses erroneously identify an innocent person. [27]

References

  1. ^ Dittman, M. "Recommendations for police lineups". American Psychological Association.
  2. ^ "Increasing Eyewitness Accuracy in Police Lineups". American Psychological Association.
  3. ^ "Where do police get the people for lineups?". The Straight Dope. November 21, 2006. Archived from the original on April 30, 2008. Retrieved July 7, 2007.
  4. ^ Lamb, Michael (2018). "Eyewitness Identification: Live, Photo, and Video Lineups". Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 24 (3): 307–325. doi: 10.1037/law0000164. PMC  6078069. PMID  30100702.
  5. ^ Campbell, Andrea; Ohm, Ralph C. (2007). Legal Ease: A Guide to Criminal Law, Evidence, and Procedure (2nd ed.). Springfield, Ill.: Charles C. Thomas Publisher. p. 176. ISBN  978-0-398-07731-0.
  6. ^ Zernike, Kate (April 19, 2006). "Study Fuels a Growing Debate Over Police Lineups". The New York Times. Retrieved July 7, 2007.
  7. ^ "Identification Procedures & Preparing the Case for Court". Administration of Justice 104. Rio Hondo College. August 2001. Retrieved July 23, 2011.
  8. ^ "Police to use virtual ID parades". BBC News. April 1, 2004. Retrieved April 18, 2012.
  9. ^ Cates, P. (2011). "Sequential lineups are more accurate, according to ground-breaking report on eyewitness identification procedures". Retrieved from http://www.innocenceproject.org
  10. ^ a b Schwartz, J. (2011). "Changes to police lineup procedures cut eyewitness mistakes, study says". The New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com
  11. ^ a b Steblay, N. K., Dysart, J. E., & Wells, G. L. (2011). "Seventy-two tests of the sequential lineup superiority effect: a meta-analysis and policy discussion". Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 17, 99–139.
  12. ^ Gronlund, S. D., Carlson, C. A., Dailey, S. B., & Goodsell, C. A. (2009). "Robustness of the sequential lineup advantage". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15(2), 140–152. doi: 10.1037/a0015082
  13. ^ Lindsay, R. L., Mansour, J. K., Beaudry, J. L., Leach, A., & Bertrand, M. I. (2009). "Beyond sequential presentation: Misconceptions and misrepresentations of sequential lineups". Legal and Criminological Psychology, 14(1), 31–34. doi: 10.1348/135532508X382104
  14. ^ Cutler, B. L., & Penrod, S. D. (1988). "Improving the reliability of eyewitness identification: Lineup construction and presentation". Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 281–299. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.73.2.281
  15. ^ Steblay, N. M., Dysart, J. E., Fulero, S. S., & Lindsay, R. L. (2002). Erratum. Law and Human Behavior, 26(4), doi: 10.1023/A:1016339523615
  16. ^ Edward J. Imwinkelried, Courtroom criminal evidence, 2nd edition, 1993, ISBN  1558341277, p. 934
  17. ^ Milt Machlin and William Read Woodfield, "Chessman Case Cracks Wide Open", Argosy, December 1960, p. 118, as quoted in Ed Cray, "Ethnic and Place Names as Derisive Adjectives", Western Folklore 21:1:27–34 (January 1962), JSTOR  1520639.
  18. ^ Mikulak, Anna (August 31, 2017). "Injecting Science into Police Lineups". Aps Observer. 30 (7).
  19. ^ Ross, David R.; Ceci, Stephen J.; Dunning, David; Toglia, Michael P. (1994). "Unconscious transference and mistaken identity: When a witness misidentifies a familiar but innocent person". Journal of Applied Psychology. 79 (6): 918–930. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.79.6.918. ISSN  1939-1854.
  20. ^ Read, J. Don (1994), Understanding bystander misidentifications: The role of familiarity and contextual knowledge, Adult Eyewitness Testimony, Cambridge University Press, pp. 56–79, ISBN  978-0-521-43255-9, retrieved 2019-12-02
  21. ^ Wells, Gary L.; Olson, Elizabeth A. (2001). "The other-race effect in eyewitness identification: What do we do about it?". Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 7 (1): 230–246. doi: 10.1037//1076-8971.7.1.230. ISSN  1076-8971.
  22. ^ Chance, J. E., & Goldstein, A. G. (1996). "The other-race effect and eyewitness identification". https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1995-99115-006
  23. ^ Myers, David G, Twenge, Jean M (2019). Social Psychology, New York: McGraw-Hill Education.[ page needed]
  24. ^ a b Goldstein, E. Bruce, Cognitive psychology : connecting mind, research, and everyday experience. ISBN  1-337-40827-1. OCLC  1055681278[ page needed]
  25. ^ a b c Wells, Gary L.; Bradfield, Amy L. (1998). "'Good, you identified the suspect': Feedback to eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience". Journal of Applied Psychology. 83 (3): 360–376. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.83.3.360. ISSN  1939-1854.
  26. ^ Malpass, Roy S.; Devine, Patricia G. (1981). "Eyewitness identification: Lineup instructions and the absence of the offender". Journal of Applied Psychology. 66 (4): 482–489. doi: 10.1037//0021-9010.66.4.482. ISSN  0021-9010.
  27. ^ a b c Colloff, Melissa F.; Wilson, Brent M.; Seale-Carlisle, Travis M.; Wixted, John T. (February 23, 2021). "Optimizing the selection of fillers in police lineups". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 118 (8): e2017292118. Bibcode: 2021PNAS..11817292C. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2017292118. ISSN  0027-8424. PMC  7923643. PMID  33593908.