Page semi-protected
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
(Redirected from Orangutans)

Orangutans
Temporal range: Early Pleistocene – Recent
O
S
D
C
P
T
J
K
N
Bornean orangutan
(Pongo pygmaeus)
Scientific classification Edit this classification
Domain: Eukaryota
Kingdom: Animalia
Phylum: Chordata
Class: Mammalia
Order: Primates
Suborder: Haplorhini
Infraorder: Simiiformes
Family: Hominidae
Subfamily: Ponginae
Genus: Pongo
Lacépède, 1799
Type species
Pongo pygmaeus
Lacépède, 1799 (Simia satyrus Linnaeus, 1760)
Species

Pongo pygmaeus
Pongo abelii
Pongo tapanuliensis
Pongo hooijeri
Pongo weidenreichi

Range of the three extant species
Synonyms

Faunus Oken, 1816
Lophotus Fischer, 1813
Macrobates Billberg, 1828
Satyrus Lesson, 1840

Orangutans are great apes native to the rainforests of Indonesia and Malaysia. They are now found only in parts of Borneo and Sumatra, but during the Pleistocene they ranged throughout Southeast Asia and South China. Classified in the genus Pongo, orangutans were originally considered to be one species. From 1996, they were divided into two species: the Bornean orangutan (P. pygmaeus, with three subspecies) and the Sumatran orangutan (P. abelii). A third species, the Tapanuli orangutan (P. tapanuliensis), was identified definitively in 2017. The orangutans are the only surviving species of the subfamily Ponginae, which diverged genetically from the other hominids ( gorillas, chimpanzees, and humans) between 19.3 and 15.7 million years ago.

The most arboreal of the great apes, orangutans spend most of their time in trees. They have proportionally long arms and short legs, and have reddish-brown hair covering their bodies. Adult males weigh about 75 kg (165 lb), while females reach about 37 kg (82 lb). Dominant adult males develop distinctive cheek pads or flanges and make long calls that attract females and intimidate rivals; younger subordinate males do not and more resemble adult females. Orangutans are the most solitary of the great apes: social bonds occur primarily between mothers and their dependent offspring. Fruit is the most important component of an orangutan's diet; but they will also eat vegetation, bark, honey, insects and bird eggs. They can live over 30 years, both in the wild and in captivity.

Orangutans are among the most intelligent primates. They use a variety of sophisticated tools and construct elaborate sleeping nests each night from branches and foliage. The apes' learning abilities have been studied extensively. There may be distinctive cultures within populations. Orangutans have been featured in literature and art since at least the 18th century, particularly in works that comment on human society. Field studies of the apes were pioneered by primatologist Birutė Galdikas and they have been kept in captive facilities around the world since at least the early 19th century.

All three orangutan species are considered critically endangered. Human activities have caused severe declines in populations and ranges. Threats to wild orangutan populations include poaching (for bushmeat and retaliation for consuming crops), habitat destruction and deforestation (for palm oil cultivation and logging), and the illegal pet trade. Several conservation and rehabilitation organisations are dedicated to the survival of orangutans in the wild.

Etymology

The name "orangutan" (also written orang-utan, orang utan, orangutang, and ourang-outang [1]) is derived from the Malay words orang, meaning "person", and hutan, meaning "forest". [2] [3] The locals originally used the name to refer to actual forest-dwelling human beings, but the word underwent a semantic extension to include apes of the Pongo genus at an early stage in the history of Malay. [2] [4]

The word orangutan appears in its older form urangutan, in a variety of premodern sources in the Old Javanese language. The earliest of these is the Kakawin Ramayana, a ninth-century or early tenth-century Javanese adaption of the Sanskrit Ramayana. In these Old Javanese sources, the word urangutan refers only to apes and not to forest-dwelling human beings. The word was not originally Javanese, but was borrowed from an early Malayic language at least a thousand years ago. Hence the ultimate origin of the term "orangutan" as denoting the Pongo ape was most likely Old Malay. [2]

In Western sources, the first printed attestation of the word for the apes is in Dutch physician Jacobus Bontius' 1631 Historiae naturalis et medicae Indiae orientalis. He reported that Malays had informed him the ape could talk, but preferred not to "lest he be compelled to labour". [5] The word appeared in several German-language descriptions of Indonesian zoology in the 17th century. It has been argued that the word comes specifically from the Banjarese variety of Malay, [4] but the age of the Old Javanese sources mentioned above make Old Malay a more likely origin for the term. Cribb and colleagues (2014) suggest that Bontius' account referred not to apes (as this description was from Java where the apes were not known from) but to humans suffering some serious medical condition (most likely cretinism) and that his use of the word was misunderstood by Nicolaes Tulp, who was the first to use the term in a publication a decade later. [6]: 10–18 

The word was first attested in English in 1693 by physician John Bulwer in the form Orang-Outang, [1] [7] and variants ending with -ng are found in many languages. This spelling (and pronunciation) has remained in use in English up to the present but has come to be regarded as incorrect. [8] [9] [10] The loss of "h" in utan and the shift from -ng to -n has been taken to suggest the term entered English through Portuguese. [4] In Malay, the term was first attested in 1840, not as an indigenous name but referring to how the English called the animal. [11] The word 'orangutan' in Malay and Indonesian today was borrowed from English or Dutch in the 20th century—explaining why the initial 'h' of 'hutan' is also missing. [4]

The name of the genus, Pongo, comes from a 16th-century account by Andrew Battel, an English sailor held prisoner by the Portuguese in Angola, which describes two anthropoid "monsters" named Pongo and Engeco. He is now believed to have been describing gorillas, but in the 18th century, the terms orangutan and pongo were used for all great apes. French naturalist Bernard Germain de Lacépède used the term Pongo for the genus in 1799. [12] [6]: 24–25  Battel's "Pongo", in turn, is from the Kongo word mpongi [13] [14] or other cognates from the region: Lumbu pungu, Vili mpungu, or Yombi yimpungu. [15]

Taxonomy and phylogeny

The orangutan was first described scientifically in 1758 in the Systema Naturae of Carl Linnaeus as Homo troglodytes. [6]: 20  It was renamed Simia pygmaeus in 1760 by his student Christian Emmanuel Hopp and given the name Pongo by Lacépède in 1799. [6]: 24–25  The populations on the two islands were suggested to be separate species when P. abelii was described by French naturalist René Lesson in 1827. [16] In 2001, P. abelii was confirmed as a full species based on molecular evidence published in 1996, [17] [18]: 53  [19] and three distinct populations on Borneo were elevated to subspecies (P. p. pygmaeus, P. p. morio and P. p. wurmbii). [20] The description in 2017 of a third species, P. tapanuliensis, from Sumatra south of Lake Toba, came with a surprising twist: it is more closely related to the Bornean species, P. pygmaeus than to its fellow Sumatran species, P. abelii. [19]

Head shots of male Bornean, Sumatran and Tapanuli orangutans
Flanged male Bornean, Sumatran and Tapanuli orangutans

The Sumatran orangutan genome was sequenced in January 2011. [21] [22] Following humans and chimpanzees, the Sumatran orangutan became the third species of great ape to have its genome sequenced. Subsequently, the Bornean species had its genome sequenced. Bornean orangutans (P. pygmaeus) have less genetic diversity than in Sumatran ones (P. abelii), despite populations being six to seven times higher in Borneo. The researchers hope these data may help conservationists preserve the endangered ape, as well as learn more about human genetic diseases. [22] Similarly to gorillas and chimpanzees, orangutans have 48  diploid chromosomes, in contrast to humans, which have 46. [23]: 30 

According to molecular evidence, within apes (superfamily Hominoidea), the gibbons diverged during the early Miocene between 24.1 and 19.7 million years ago (mya), and the orangutans diverged from the African great ape lineage between 19.3 and 15.7 mya. Israfil and colleagues (2011) estimated based on mitochondrial, Y-linked, and X-linked loci that the Sumatran and Bornean species diverged 4.9 to 2.9 mya. [24]: Fig. 4  By contrast, the 2011 genome study suggested that these two species diverged as recently as circa 400,000 years ago. The study also found that orangutans evolved at a slower pace than both chimpanzees and humans. [22] A 2017 genome study found that the Bornean and Tapanuli orangutans diverged from Sumatran orangutans about 3.4 mya, and from each other around 2.4 mya. Millions of years ago, orangutans travelled from mainland Asia to Sumatra and then Borneo as the islands were connected by land bridges during the recent glacial periods when sea levels were much lower. The present range of Tapanuli orangutans is thought to be close to where ancestral orangutans first entered what is now Indonesia from mainland Asia. [19] [25]

Taxonomy of genus Pongo [26] Phylogeny of superfamily Hominoidea [24]: Fig. 4 
Genus Pongo
  • Bornean orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus)
    • Pongo pygmaeus pygmaeus – northwest populations
    • Pongo pygmaeus morio – east populations
    • Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii – southwest populations
  • Sumatran orangutan (Pongo abelii – Sumatra northwest of Lake Toba)
  • Tapanuli orangutan (Pongo tapanuliensis – Sumatra south of Lake Toba)
 Hominoidea

gibbons (family Hylobatidae)

orangutans (genus Pongo)

gorillas (genus Gorilla)

chimpanzees (genus Pan)

humans (genus  Homo)

Partial fossil skull of ape
Fossil skull of Sivapithecus sivalensis, an extinct relative of orangutan

Fossil record

The three orangutan species are the only extant members of the subfamily Ponginae. This subfamily also includes extinct apes such as Lufengpithecus, which occurred 8–2  mya in southern China and Thailand; [18]: 50  Indopithecus, which lived in India from 9.2 to 8.6 mya; and Sivapithecus, which lived in India and Pakistan from 12.5 mya until 8.5 mya. [27] These animals likely lived in drier and cooler environments than orangutans do today. Khoratpithecus piriyai, which lived 5–7 mya in Thailand, is believed to be the closest known relative of the living orangutans and inhabited similar environments. [18]: 50  The largest known primate, Gigantopithecus, was also a member of Ponginae and lived in China, from 2 mya to 300,000 years ago. [28] [18]: 50 

The oldest known record of Pongo is from the Early Pleistocene of Chongzuo, consisting of teeth ascribed to extinct species P. weidenreichi. [29] [30] Pongo is found as part of the faunal complex in the Pleistocene cave assemblage in Vietnam, alongside Giganopithecus, though it is known only from teeth. Some fossils described under the name P. hooijeri have been found in Vietnam, and multiple fossil subspecies have been described from several parts of southeastern Asia. It is unclear if these belong to P. pygmaeus or P. abelii or, in fact, represent distinct species. [31] During the Pleistocene, Pongo had a far more extensive range than at present, extending throughout Sundaland and mainland Southeast Asia and South China. Teeth of orangutans are known from Peninsular Malaysia that date to 60,000 years ago. [32] The youngest remains from South China, which are teeth assigned to P. weidenreichi, date to between 66 and 57,000 years ago. [33] The range of orangutans had contracted significantly by the end of the Pleistocene, most likely because of the reduction of forest habitat during the Last Glacial Maximum. Though they may have survived into the Holocene in Cambodia and Vietnam. [29] [32]

Characteristics

Head and shoulder shots of an adult male and female orangutan
Adult male (left) and female Tapanuli orangutans

Orangutans display significant sexual dimorphism; females typically stand 115 cm (3 ft 9 in) tall and weigh around 37 kg (82 lb), while adult males stand 137 cm (4 ft 6 in) tall and weigh 75 kg (165 lb). Compared to humans, they have proportionally long arms, a male orangutan having an arm span of about 2 m (6.6 ft), and short legs. They are covered in long reddish hair that starts out bright orange and darkens to maroon or chocolate with age, while the skin is grey-black. Though largely hairless, males' faces can develop some hair, giving them a beard. [34] [35] [18]: 13–15 

Orangutans have small ears and noses; the ears are unlobed. [34] The mean endocranial volume is 397 cm3. [36] The cranium is elevated relative to the face, which is incurved and prognathous. [34] Compared to chimpanzees and gorillas, the brow ridge of an orangutan is underdeveloped. [37] Females and juveniles have relatively circular skulls and thin faces while mature males have a prominent sagittal crest, large cheek pads or flanges, [34] extensive throat pouches and long canines. [34] [18]: 14  The cheek pads are made mostly of fatty tissue and are supported by the musculature of the face. [38] The throat pouches act as resonance chambers for making long calls. [39]

An orangutan skeleton
Skeleton of subadult Bornean orangutan

Orangutan hands have four long fingers but a dramatically shorter opposable thumb for a strong grip on branches as they travel high in the trees. The resting configuration of the fingers is curved, creating a suspensory hook grip. With the thumb out of the way, the fingers (and hands) can grip securely around objects with a small diameter by resting the tops of the fingers against the inside of the palm, thus creating a double-locked grip. [40] Their feet have four long toes and an opposable big toe, giving them hand-like dexterity. The hip joints also allow for their legs to rotate similarly to their arms and shoulders. [18]: 15 

Orangutans move through the trees by both vertical climbing and suspension. Compared to other great apes, they infrequently descend to the ground where they are more cumbersome. Unlike gorillas and chimpanzees, orangutans are not true knuckle-walkers, instead bending their digits and walking on the sides of their hands and feet. [41] [42]

Compared to their relatives in Borneo, Sumatran orangutans are more slender with paler and longer hair and a longer face. [35] Tapanuli orangutans resemble Sumatran orangutans more than Bornean orangutans in body build and hair colour. [19] They have shaggier hair, smaller skulls, and flatter faces than the other two species. [43]

Ecology and behaviour

Wild orangutan in the Danum Valley ( Sabah, Malaysia, Borneo island)

Orangutans are mainly arboreal and inhabit tropical rainforest, particularly lowland dipterocarp and old secondary forest. [35] [44] Populations are more concentrated near riverside habitats, such as freshwater and peat swamp forest, while drier forests away from the flooded areas have fewer apes. Population density also decreases at higher elevations. [23]: 92  Orangutans occasionally enter grasslands, cultivated fields, gardens, young secondary forest, and shallow lakes. [44]

Most of the day is spent feeding, resting, and travelling. [45] They start the day feeding for two to three hours in the morning. They rest during midday, then travel in the late afternoon. When evening arrives, they prepare their nests for the night. [44] Potential predators of orangutans include tigers, clouded leopards and wild dogs. [23]: 91  The most common orangutan parasites are nematodes of the genus Strongyloides and the ciliate Balantidium coli. Among Strongyloides, the species S. fuelleborni and S. stercoralis are reported in young individuals. [46] Orangutans also use the plant species Dracaena cantleyi as an anti-inflammatory balm. [47] Captive animals may suffer an upper respiratory tract disease. [48]

Diet and feeding

Orangutan on a branch eating some leaves
Although orangutans may consume leaves, shoots, and bird eggs, fruit is the most important part of their diet.

Orangutans are primarily fruit-eaters, which can take up 57–80% of their foraging time. Even during times of scarcity, fruit is 16% of their feeding time. Fruits with soft pulp, arils or seed-walls are consumed the most, particularly figs but also drupes and berries. [23]: 65  Orangutans are thought to be the sole fruit disperser for some plant species including the vine species Strychnos ignatii which contains the toxic alkaloid strychnine. [49]

Orangutans also include leaves in their diet, which take up 25% of their average foraging time. Leaves are eaten more when fruit is less available, but even during times of fruit abundance, orangutans will eat leaves 11–20% of the time. They appear to depend on the leaf and stem material of Borassodendron borneensis during times of low fruit abundance. Other food items consumed by the apes include bark, honey, bird eggs, insects and small vertebrates including slow lorises. [44] [23]: 65–66 

In some areas, orangutans may practise geophagy, which involves consuming soil and other earth substances. They will uproot soil from the ground as well as eat shelter tubes from tree trunks. Orangutans also visit the sides of cliffs or earth depressions for their mineral licks. Orangutans may eat soils for their anti-toxic kaolin minerals, since their diet contains toxic tannins and phenolic acids. [23]: 67 

Social life

Two orangutans swinging on tree branches
Orangutans are the least social of the great apes.

The social structure of the orangutan can be best described as solitary but social; they live a more solitary lifestyle than the other great apes. [50] Bornean orangutans are generally more solitary than Sumatran orangutans. [35] Most social bonds occur between adult females and their dependent and weaned offspring. Resident females live with their offspring in defined home ranges that overlap with those of other adult females, which may be their immediate relatives. One to several resident female home ranges are encompassed within the home range of a resident male, who is their main mating partner. [50] [51] Interactions between adult females range from friendly to avoidance to antagonistic. [52] Flanged males are hostile to both other flanged males and unflanged males, while unflanged males are more peaceful towards each other. [53]

Orangutans disperse and establish their home ranges by age 11. Females tend to live near their birth range, while males disperse farther but may still visit their birth range within their larger home range. [51] [54] They enter a transient phase, which lasts until a male can challenge and displace a dominant, resident male from his home range. [55] Both resident and transient orangutans aggregate on large fruiting trees to feed. The fruits tend to be abundant, so competition is low and individuals may engage in social interactions. [56] [57] [58] Orangutans will also form travelling groups with members moving between different food sources. [55] They are often consortships between an adult male and a female. [56] Social grooming is uncommon among orangutans. [59]

Communication

Orangutans communicate with various vocals and sounds. Males will make long calls, both to attract females and to advertise themselves to other males. [39] These calls have three components; they begin with grumbles, peak with pulses and end with bubbles. Both sexes will try to intimidate conspecifics with a series of low frequency noises known collectively as the "rolling call". When uncomfortable, an orangutan will produce a "kiss squeak", which involves sucking in air through pursed lips. Mothers produce throatscrapes to keep in contact with their offspring. Infants make soft hoots when distressed. When building a nest, orangutans will produce smacks or blow raspberries. [60] Orangutan calls display consonant- and vowel-like components and they maintain their meaning over great distances. [61]

Mother orangutans and offspring also use several different gestures and expressions such as beckoning, stomping, lower lip pushing, object shaking and "presenting" a body part. These communicate goals such as "acquire object", "climb on me", "climb on you", "climb over", "move away", "play change: decrease intensity", "resume play" and "stop that". [62]

Reproduction and development

Flanged male orangutan
Unflanged male orangutan

Males become sexually mature at around age 15. They may exhibit arrested development by not developing the distinctive cheek pads, pronounced throat pouches, long fur, or long calls until a resident dominant male is absent. The transformation from unflanged to flanged can occur quickly. Flanged males attract females in oestrous with their characteristic long calls, which may also suppress development in younger males. [39] [18]: 100 

Unflanged males wander widely in search of oestrous females and upon finding one, will force copulation on her, the occurrence of which is unusually high among mammals. Females prefer to mate with the fitter flanged males, forming pairs with them and benefiting from their protection. [63] [55] [64] Non- ovulating females do not usually resist copulation with unflanged males, as the chance of conception is low. [64] Homosexual behaviour has been recorded in the context of both affiliative and aggressive interactions. [65]

A mother orangutan with her offspring
Mother orangutan with young

Unlike females of other non-human great ape species, orangutans do not exhibit sexual swellings to signal fertility. [64] A female first gives birth around 15 years of age and they have a six to nine year interbirth interval, the longest among the great apes. [66] Gestation is around nine months long and infants are born at a weight of 1.5–2 kg (3.3–4.4 lb). [18]: 99  Usually only a single infant is born; twins are a rare occurrence. [67] Unlike many other primates, male orangutans do not seem to practise infanticide. This may be because they cannot ensure they will sire a female's next offspring, because she does not immediately begin ovulating again after her infant dies. [68] There is evidence that females with offspring under six years old generally avoid adult males. [69]

Females do most of the caring of the young. The mother will carry the infant while travelling, suckle it and sleep with it. [18]: 100  During its first four months, the infant is almost never without physical contact and clings to its mother's belly. In the following months, the amount of physical contact the infant has with its mother declines. When an orangutan reaches the age of one-and-a-half years, its climbing skills improve and it will travel through the canopy holding hands with other orangutans, a behaviour known as "buddy travel". [59] After two years of age, juvenile orangutans will begin to move away from their mothers temporarily. They reach adolescence at six or seven years of age and are able to live alone but retain some connections with their mothers. [18]: 100  Females may nurse their offspring for up to eight years, which is more than any mammal. [70] Typically, orangutans live over 30 years both in the wild and in captivity. [18]: 15 

Nesting

Orangutan lying on its back in a nest
An orangutan lying in its nest

Orangutans build nests specialised for either day or night use. These are carefully constructed; young orangutans learn from observing their mother's nest-building behaviour. In fact, nest-building allows young orangutans to become less dependent on their mother. From six months of age onwards, orangutans practise nest-building and gain proficiency by the time they are three years old. [71]

Construction of a night nest is done by following a sequence of steps. Initially, a suitable tree is located. Orangutans are choosy about sites, though nests can be found in many tree species. To establish a foundation, the ape grabs the large branches under it and bends them so they join. The orangutan then does the same to smaller, leafier branches to create a "mattress". After this, the ape stands and braids the tips of branches into the mattress. Doing this increases the stability of the nest. Orangutans make their nests more comfortable by creating "pillows", "blankets", "roofs" and "bunk-beds". [71]

Intelligence

An orangutan imitating human speech [72]

Orangutans are among the most intelligent non-human primates. Experiments suggest they can track the displacement of objects both visible and hidden. [73] [74] Zoo Atlanta has a touch-screen computer on which their two Sumatran orangutans play games. [75] A 2008 study of two orangutans at the Leipzig Zoo showed orangutans may practise "calculated reciprocity", which involves an individual aiding another with the expectation of being paid back. Orangutans are the first nonhuman species documented to do so. [76]

In a 1997 study, two captive adult orangutans were tested with the cooperative pulling paradigm. Without any training, the orangutans succeeded in pulling off an object to get food in the first session. Over the course of 30 sessions, the apes succeeded more quickly, having learned to coordinate. [77] An adult orangutan has been documented to pass the mirror test, indicating self-awareness. [78] Mirror tests with a 2-year-old orangutan failed to reveal self-recognition. [79]

Studies in the wild indicate that flanged male orangutans plan their movements in advance and signal them to other individuals. [80] Experiments have also suggested that orangutans can communicate about things that are not present: mother orangutans remain silent in the presence of a perceived threat but when it passes, the mother produces an alarm call to their offspring to teach them about the danger. [81] Orangutans and other great apes show laughter-like vocalisations in response to physical contact such as wrestling, play chasing or tickling. This suggests that laughter derived from a common origin among primate species and therefore evolved before the origin of humans. [82] Orangutans can learn to mimic new sounds by purposely controlling the vibrations of their vocal folds, a trait that led to speech in humans. [72] [83] Bonnie, an orangutan at the US National Zoo, was recorded spontaneously whistling after hearing a caretaker. She appears to whistle without expecting a food reward. [84]

Tool use and culture

An orangutan using a stick to pick at a hole in a rock with a cup of orange-juice concentrate.
An orangutan at the San Diego Zoo using a tool to extract orange-juice concentrate

Tool use in orangutans was observed by primatologist Birutė Galdikas in ex-captive populations. [85] Orangutans in Suaq Balimbing were recorded to develop a tool kit for use in foraging which consisted of both insect-extraction sticks for use in the hollows of trees and seed-extraction sticks for harvesting seeds from hard-husked fruit. The orangutans adjusted their tools according to the task at hand, and preference was given to oral tool use. [86] [87] This preference was also found in an experimental study of captive orangutans. [88] Orangutans have been observed to use sticks to poke at catfish, causing them to leap out of the water so the orangutan can grab them. [89] [90] Orangutan have also been documented to keep tools for later. [91] When building a nest, orangutans appear to be able to determine which branches would better support their body weight. [92]

Primatologist Carel P. van Schaik and biological anthropologist Cheryl D. Knott further investigated tool use in different wild orangutan populations. They compared geographic variations in tool use related to the processing of Neesia fruit. The orangutans of Suaq Balimbing were found to be avid users of insect and seed-extraction tools when compared to other wild orangutans. [93] [94] The scientists suggested these differences are cultural as they do not correlate with habitat. The orangutans at Suaq Balimbing are closely spaced and relatively tolerant of each other; this creates favourable conditions for the spreading of new behaviours. [93] Further evidence that highly social orangutans are more likely to exhibit cultural behaviours came from a study of leaf-carrying behaviours of formerly captive orangutans that were being rehabilitated on the island of Kaja in Borneo. [95]

Wild orangutans in Tuanan, Borneo, were reported to use tools in acoustic communication. They use leaves to amplify the kiss squeak sounds they produce. The apes may employ this method of amplification to deceive the listener into believing they are larger animals. [96] In 2003, researchers from six different orangutan field sites who used the same behavioural coding scheme compared the behaviours of the animals from each site. They found each orangutan population used different tools. The evidence suggested the differences were cultural: first, the extent of the differences increased with distance, suggesting cultural diffusion was occurring, and second, the size of the orangutans' cultural repertoire increased according to the amount of social contact present within the group. Social contact facilitates cultural transmission. [97]

Personhood

In June 2008, Spain would become the first country to recognise the rights of some non-human great apes, based on the guidelines of the Great Ape Project, which are that chimpanzees, bonobos, orangutans, and gorillas not to be used for animal experiments. [98] [99] In December 2014, a court in Argentina ruled that an orangutan named Sandra at the Buenos Aires Zoo must be moved to a sanctuary in Brazil to provide her "partial or controlled freedom". Sandra has since been relocated to The Center for Great Apes in the United States, as it is the only accredited orangutan sanctuary in the Americas. [100] Animal rights groups like Great Ape Project Argentina argued the ruling should apply to all species in captivity, and legal specialists from the Argentina's Federal Chamber of Criminal Cassatio considered the ruling applicable only to non-human hominids. [101]

Orangutans and humans

Birutė Galdikas shown speaking into a microphone
Orangutan researcher Birutė Galdikas presenting her book about the apes

Orangutans were known to the native people of Sumatra and Borneo for millennia. The apes are known as maias in Sarawak and mawas in other parts of Borneo and in Sumatra. [11] While some communities hunted them for food and decoration, others placed taboos on such practices. In central Borneo, some traditional folk beliefs consider it bad luck to look an orangutan in the face. Some folk tales involve orangutans mating with and kidnapping humans. There are even stories of hunters being captured by female orangutans. [18]: 66–71 

Europeans became aware of the existence of the orangutan in the 17th century. [18]: 60  Explorers in Borneo hunted them extensively during the 19th century. In 1779, Dutch anatomist Petrus Camper, who observed the animals and dissected some specimens, gave the first scientific description of the orangutan. [18]: 64–65  Camper mistakenly thought that flanged and unflanged male orangutans were different species, a misconception corrected after his death. [102]

Little was known about orangutan behaviour until the field studies of Birutė Galdikas, [103] who became a leading authority on the apes. When she arrived in Borneo in 1971, Galdikas settled into a primitive bark-and-thatch hut at a site she dubbed Camp Leakey, in Tanjung Puting. She studied orangutans for the next four years and developed her PhD thesis for UCLA. [104] Galdikas became an outspoken advocate for orangutans and the preservation of their rainforest habitat, which is rapidly being devastated by loggers, palm oil plantations, gold miners, and unnatural forest fires. [105] Along with Jane Goodall and Dian Fossey, Galdikas is considered to be one of Leakey's Angels, named after anthropologist Louis Leakey. [106]

In fiction

Illustration of an orangutan attacking a woman from The Murders in the Rue Morgue by Daniel Vierge
1870 illustration for " The Murders in the Rue Morgue" by Daniel Vierge

Orangutans first appeared in Western fiction in the 18th century and have been used to comment on human society. Written by the pseudonymous A. Ardra, Tintinnabulum naturae (The Bell of Nature, 1772) is told from the point of view of a human-orangutan hybrid who calls himself the "metaphysician of the woods". Around 50 years later, the anonymously written work The Orang Outang is narrated by a pure orangutan in captivity in the US, writing a letter critiquing Boston society to her friend in Java. [6]: 108–09 

Thomas Love Peacock's 1817 novel Melincourt features Sir Oran Haut Ton, an orangutan who lives among English people and becomes a candidate for Member of Parliament. The novel satirises the class and political system of Britain. Oran's purity and status as a "natural man" stand in contrast to the immorality and corruption of the "civilised" humans. [6]: 110–11  In Frank Challice Constable's The Curse of Intellect (1895), the protagonist Reuben Power travels to Borneo and captures an orangutan to train it to speak so he can "know what a beast like that might think of us". [6]: 114–15  Orangutans are featured prominently in the 1963 science fiction novel Planet of the Apes by Pierre Boulle and the media franchise derived from it. They are typically portrayed as bureaucrats like Dr. Zaius, the science minister. [6]: 118–19, 175–76 

Orangutans are sometimes portrayed as antagonists, notably in the 1832 Walter Scott novel Count Robert of Paris and the 1841 Edgar Allan Poe short story The Murders in the Rue Morgue. [6]: 145  Disney's 1967 animated musical adaptation of The Jungle Book added a jazzy orangutan named King Louie, who tries to get Mowgli to teach him how to make fire. [6]: 266  The 1986 horror film Link features an intelligent orangutan which serves a university professor but has sinister motives; he plots against humanity and stalks a female student assistant. [6]: 174–75  Other stories have portrayed orangutans helping humans, such as The Librarian in Terry Pratchett's fantasy novels Discworld and in Dale Smith's 2004 novel What the Orangutan Told Alice. [6]: 123  More comical portrayals of the orangutan include the 1996 film Dunston Checks In. [6]: 181 

In captivity

Sketch of the female orangutan known as Jenny sitting in a chair
The Female Orang – Utan ( Jenny sitting in a chair) [107] c. 1830s

By the early 19th century, orangutans were being kept in captivity. In 1817, an orangutan joined several other animals in London's Exeter Exchange. The ape was recorded to have rejected the company of other animals, aside from a dog, and preferred to be with humans. He was occasionally taken on coach rides clothed in a smock-frock and hat and even given drinks at an inn where he behaved politely for the hosts. [6]: 64–65  The London Zoo housed a female orangutan named Jenny who was dressed in human clothing and learned to drink tea. She is remembered for her meeting with Charles Darwin who compared her reactions to those of a human child. [107] [108]

Zoos and circuses in the Western world would continue to use orangutans and other simians as sources for entertainment, training them to behave like humans at tea parties and to perform tricks. Notable orangutan "character actors" include: Jacob and Rosa of the Tierpark Hagenbeck in Hamburg, Germany, in the early 20th century; Joe Martin of Universal City Zoo in the 1910s and 1920s; and Jiggs of the San Diego Zoo in the 1930s and 1940s. [6]: 187–89, 193–94  [109] Animal rights groups have urged a stop to such acts, considering them abusive. [110] Starting in the 1960s, zoos became more concerned with education and orangutans exhibits were designed to mimic their natural environment and displayed their natural behaviours. [6]: 185, 206  Ken Allen, an orangutan of the San Diego Zoo, became world famous in the 1980s for multiple escapes from his enclosures. He was nicknamed "the hairy Houdini" and was the subject of a fan club, T-shirts, bumper stickers and a song titled The Ballad of Ken Allen. [111]

Galdikas reported that her cook was sexually assaulted by a captive male orangutan. [112] The ape may have suffered from a skewed species identity and forced copulation is a standard mating strategy for low-ranking male orangutans. [113] American animal trafficker Frank Buck claimed to have seen human mothers acting as wet nurses to orphaned orangutan babies in hopes of keeping them alive long enough to sell to a trader, which would be an instance of human–animal breastfeeding. [114]

Conservation

Status and threats

All three species are critically endangered according to the IUCN Red List of mammals. [115] [116] [117] They are legally protected from capture, harm or killing in both Malaysia and Indonesia, [118] and are listed under Appendix I by CITES, which prohibits their unlicensed trade under international law. [119] The Bornean orangutan range has become more fragmented, with few or no apes documented in the southeast. [116] The largest remaining population is found in the forest around the Sabangau River, but this environment is at risk. [120] The Sumatran orangutan is found only in the northern part of Sumatra, most of the population inhabiting the Leuser Ecosystem. [115] The Tapanuli orangutan is found only in the Batang Toru forest of Sumatra. [117]

A view of deforested land
Deforestation for palm oil production in Indonesia

Birutė Galdikas wrote that orangutans were already threatened by poaching and deforestation when she began studying them in 1971. [121] By the 2000s, orangutan habitats decreased rapidly because of logging, mining and fragmentation by roads. A major factor has been the conversion of vast areas of tropical forest to palm oil plantations in response to international demand. Hunting is also a major problem, as is the illegal pet trade. [115] [116]

Orangutans may be killed for the bushmeat trade [122] and bones are secretly sold in souvenir shops in several cities in Indonesian Borneo. [123] Conflicts between locals and orangutans also pose a threat. Orangutans that have lost their homes often raid agricultural areas and end up being killed by villagers. [124] Locals may also be motivated to kill orangutans for food or because of their perceived danger. [125] Mother orangutans are killed so their infants can be sold as pets. Between 2012 and 2017, the Indonesian authorities, with the aid of the Orangutan Information Center, seized 114 orangutans, 39 of which were pets. [126]

Estimates in the 2000s found that around 6,500 Sumatran orangutans and around 54,000 Bornean orangutans remain in the wild. [127] A 2016 study estimates a population of 14,613 Sumatran orangutans in the wild, twice that of previous population estimates, [128] while 2016 estimates suggest 104,700 Bornean orangutans exist in the wild. [116] A 2018 study found that Bornean orangutans declined by 148,500 individuals from 1999 to 2015. [129] Fewer than 800 Tapanuli orangutans are estimated to still exist, which puts the species among the most endangered of the great apes. [130] [43]

Conservation centres and organisations

Peter Pratje with an orangutan
Zoologische Gesellschaft Frankfurt Programm Director Peter Pratje works with orangutans in Bukit Tigapuluh, Indonesia.

Several organisations are working for the rescue, rehabilitation and reintroduction of orangutans. The largest of these is the Borneo Orangutan Survival (BOS) Foundation, founded by conservationist Willie Smits and which operates projects such as the Nyaru Menteng Rehabilitation Program founded by conservationist Lone Drøscher Nielsen. [131] [132] [133]

A female orangutan was rescued from a village brothel in Kareng Pangi village, Central Kalimantan, in 2003. The orangutan was shaved and chained for sexual purposes. Since being freed, the orangutan, named Pony, has been living with the BOS. She has been re-socialised to live with other orangutans. [134] In May 2017, the BOS rescued an albino orangutan from captivity in a remote village in Kapuas Hulu, on the island of Kalimantan in Indonesian Borneo. According to volunteers at BOS, albino orangutans are extremely rare (one in ten thousand). This is the first albino orangutan the organisation has seen in 25 years of activity. [135]

Other major conservation centres in Indonesia include those at Tanjung Puting National Park, Sebangau National Park, Gunung Palung National Park and Bukit Baka Bukit Raya National Park in Borneo and the Gunung Leuser National Park and Bukit Lawang in Sumatra. In Malaysia, conservation areas include Semenggoh Wildlife Centre and Matang Wildlife Centre also in Sarawak, and the Sepilok Orang Utan Sanctuary in Sabah. [136] Major conservation centres headquartered outside the orangutans' home countries include Frankfurt Zoological Society, [137] Orangutan Foundation International, which was founded by Galdikas, [138] and the Australian Orangutan Project. [139] Conservation organisations such as the Orangutan Land Trust work with the palm oil industry to improve sustainability and encourages the industry to establish conservation areas for orangutans. [140] [141]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b "orangutan, n.". Oxford English Dictionary. March 2022. Retrieved 4 August 2022.
  2. ^ a b c Sastrawan, Wayan Jarrah (2020). "The Word 'Orangutan': Old Malay Origin or European Concoction". Bijdragen tot de Land-, Taal- en Volkenkunde. 176 (4): 532–41. doi: 10.1163/22134379-bja10016. S2CID  228828226. Archived from the original on 12 April 2021. Retrieved 12 April 2021.
  3. ^ Harper, Douglas. "Orangutan". Online Etymology Dictionary. Archived from the original on 4 November 2012. Retrieved 4 May 2012.
  4. ^ a b c d Mahdi, Waruno (2007). Malay Words and Malay Things: Lexical Souvenirs from an Exotic Archipelago in German Publications Before 1700. Frankfurter Forschungen zu Südostasien. Vol. 3. Otto Harrassowitz Verlag. pp. 170–81. ISBN  978-3-447-05492-8.
  5. ^ Dellios, Paulette (2008). "A lexical odyssey from the Malay World". Studia Universitatis Petru Maior. Philologia. 4 (4): 141–44.
  6. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Cribb, Robert; Gilbert, Helen; Tiffin, Helen (2014). Wild Man from Borneo: A Cultural History of the Orangutan. University of Hawai'i Press. ISBN  978-0-8248-3714-3.
  7. ^ Bulwer, John (1653). Anthropometamorphosis: man transform'd: or, The artificiall changling, historically presented, in the mad and cruel gallantry, foolish bravery, ridiculous beauty, filthy finenesse, and loathsome loveliness of most nations, fashioning and altering their bodies from the mould intended by nature; with figures of those transformations. To which artificial and affected deformations are added, all the native and national monstrosities that have appeared to disfigure the humane fabrick. With a vindication of the regular beauty and honesty of nature. Internet Archive (2nd ed.). London: W. Hunt. p. 437.
  8. ^ "Orangutan". Alpha Dictionary. Archived from the original on 7 July 2016. Retrieved 20 December 2006.
  9. ^ Tan, Peter (October 1998). "Malay loan words across different dialects of English". English Today. 14 (4): 44–50. doi: 10.1017/S026607840001052X. S2CID  144326996.
  10. ^ Cannon, Garland (1992). "Malay(sian) borrowings in English". American Speech. 67 (2): 134–62. doi: 10.2307/455451. JSTOR  455451.
  11. ^ a b Rubis, June Mary (2020). "The orang utan is not an indigenous name: knowing and naming the maias as a decolonizing epistemology". Cultural Studies. 34 (5): 811–30. doi: 10.1080/09502386.2020.1780281. S2CID  221192990.
  12. ^ Groves, Colin P. (2002). "A history of gorilla taxonomy" (PDF). In Taylor, Andrea B.; Goldsmith, Michele L. (eds.). Gorilla Biology: A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Cambridge University Press. pp. 15–34. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511542558.004. ISBN  978-0521792813. Archived from the original (PDF) on 26 March 2009.
  13. ^ "pongo". Etymology Online. Archived from the original on 5 December 2018. Retrieved 4 December 2018.
  14. ^ "pongo". Merriam-Webster. Archived from the original on 5 December 2018. Retrieved 4 October 2018.
  15. ^ "pongo, n.1". OED Online. Oxford University Press. Archived from the original on 19 August 2021. Retrieved 4 October 2018.
  16. ^ Lesson, René-Primevère (1827). Manuel de mammalogie ou Histoire naturelle des mammifères (in French). Roret, Libraire. p.  32.
  17. ^ Xu, X.; Arnason, U. (1996). "The mitochondrial DNA molecule of sumatran orangutan and a molecular proposal for two (Bornean and Sumatran) species of orangutan". Journal of Molecular Evolution. 43 (5): 431–37. Bibcode: 1996JMolE..43..431X. doi: 10.1007/BF02337514. PMID  8875856. S2CID  3355899.
  18. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o Payne, J; Prundente, C (2008). Orangutans: Behaviour, Ecology and Conservation. New Holland Publishers. ISBN  978-0-262-16253-1.
  19. ^ a b c d Nater, A.; Mattle-Greminger, M. P.; Nurcahyo, A.; Nowak, M. G.; et al. (2 November 2017). "Morphometric, Behavioral, and Genomic Evidence for a New Orangutan Species". Current Biology. 27 (22): 3487–3498.e10. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.047. hdl: 10230/34400. PMID  29103940.
  20. ^ Bradon-Jones, D.; Eudey, A. A.; Geissmann, T.; Groves, C. P.; Melnick, D. J.; Morales, J. C.; Shekelle, M.; Stewart, C. B. (2004). "Asian primate classification" (PDF). International Journal of Primatology. 25: 97–164. doi: 10.1023/B:IJOP.0000014647.18720.32. S2CID  29045930. Archived (PDF) from the original on 6 December 2010. Retrieved 1 May 2011.
  21. ^ Locke, D. P.; Hillier, L. W.; Warren, W. C.; Worley, K. C.; Nazareth, L. V.; Muzny, D. M.; Yang, S. P.; Wang, Z.; Chinwalla, A. T.; Minx, P.; Mitreva, M.; Cook, L.; Delehaunty, K. D.; Fronick, C.; Schmidt, H.; Fulton, L. A.; Fulton, R. S.; Nelson, J. O.; Magrini, V.; Pohl, C.; Graves, T. A.; Markovic, C.; Cree, A.; Dinh, H. H.; Hume, J.; Kovar, C. L.; Fowler, G. R.; Lunter, G.; Meader, S.; et al. (2011). "Comparative and demographic analysis of orang-utan genomes". Nature. 469 (7331): 529–33. Bibcode: 2011Natur.469..529L. doi: 10.1038/nature09687. PMC  3060778. PMID  21270892.
  22. ^ a b c Singh, Ranjeet (26 January 2011). "Orang-utans join the genome gang". Nature. doi: 10.1038/news.2011.50. Archived from the original on 27 January 2011. Retrieved 27 January 2011.
  23. ^ a b c d e f Rijksen H. D.; Meijaard, E. (1999). Our vanishing relative: the status of wild orang-utans at the close of the twentieth century. Springer. ISBN  978-0792357551.
  24. ^ a b Israfil, H.; Zehr, S. M.; Mootnick, A. R.; Ruvolo, M.; Steiper, M. E. (2011). "Unresolved molecular phylogenies of gibbons and siamangs (Family: Hylobatidae) based on mitochondrial, Y-linked, and X-linked loci indicate a rapid Miocene radiation or sudden vicariance event" (PDF). Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 58 (3): 447–55. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2010.11.005. PMC  3046308. PMID  21074627. Archived from the original (PDF) on 10 May 2012.
  25. ^ Goldman, Jason G. (2 November 2017). "New Species of Orangutan Is Rarest Great Ape on Earth". National Geographic Society. Archived from the original on 23 July 2018. Retrieved 6 November 2017.
  26. ^ Groves, C. P. (2005). Wilson, D. E.; Reeder, D. M. (eds.). Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 183–84. ISBN  0-801-88221-4. OCLC  62265494.
  27. ^ Bhandari, A.; Kay, R. F.; Williams, B. A.; Tiwari, B. N.; Bajpai, S.; Heironymus, T. (2018). "First record of the Miocene hominoid Sivapithecus from Kutch, Gujarat state, western India". PLOS ONE. 13 (11): 10.1371/journal.pone.0206314. Bibcode: 2018PLoSO..1306314B. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206314. PMC  6235281. PMID  30427876.
  28. ^ Zhang, Yingqi; Harrison, Terry (2017). "Gigantopithecus blacki: a giant ape from the Pleistocene of Asia revisited". Yearbook of Physical Anthropology. 162 (S63): 153–77. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.23150. PMID  28105715.
  29. ^ a b Harrison, Terry; Jin, Changzhu; Zhang, Yingqi; Wang, Yuan; Zhu, Min (December 2014). "Fossil Pongo from the Early Pleistocene Gigantopithecus fauna of Chongzuo, Guangxi, southern China". Quaternary International. 354: 59–67. Bibcode: 2014QuInt.354...59H. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2014.01.013.
  30. ^ Wang, Cui-Bin; Zhao, Ling-Xia; Jin, Chang-Zhu; Wang, Yuan; Qin, Da-Gong; Pan, Wen-Shi (December 2014). "New discovery of Early Pleistocene orangutan fossils from Sanhe Cave in Chongzuo, Guangxi, southern China". Quaternary International. 354: 68–74. Bibcode: 2014QuInt.354...68W. doi: 10.1016/j.quaint.2014.06.020.
  31. ^ Schwartz, J. H.; Vu The Long; Nguyen Lan Cuong; Le Trung Kha; Tattersall, I. (1995). "A review of the Pleistocene hominoid fauna of the Socialist Republic of Vietnam (excluding Hylobatidae)". Anthropological Papers of the American Museum of Natural History (76): 1–24. hdl: 2246/259.
  32. ^ a b Ibrahim, Yasamin Kh.; Tshen, Lim Tze; Westaway, Kira E.; Cranbrook, Earl of; Humphrey, Louise; Muhammad, Ros Fatihah; Zhao, Jian-xin; Peng, Lee Chai (December 2013). "First discovery of Pleistocene orangutan (Pongo sp.) fossils in Peninsular Malaysia: Biogeographic and paleoenvironmental implications". Journal of Human Evolution. 65 (6): 770–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2013.09.005. PMID  24210657.
  33. ^ Liao, Wei; Harrison, Terry; Yao, Yanyan; Liang, Hua; Tian, Chun; Feng, Yuexing; Li, Sheng; Bae, Christopher J.; Wang, Wei (September 2022). "Evidence for the latest fossil Pongo in southern China". Journal of Human Evolution. 170: 103233. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2022.103233. PMID  36030625. S2CID  251879262.
  34. ^ a b c d e Groves, Colin P. (1971). "Pongo pygmaeus". Mammalian Species (4): 1–6. doi: 10.2307/3503852. JSTOR  3503852.
  35. ^ a b c d van Schaik, C.; MacKinnon, J. (2001). "Orangutans". In MacDonald, D. (ed.). The Encyclopedia of Mammals (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press. pp.  420–23. ISBN  978-0-87196-871-5.
  36. ^ Aiello, L.; Dean, C. (1990). An Introduction to Human Evolutionary Anatomy. Academic Press. p. 193. ISBN  0-12-045590-0.
  37. ^ Hilloowala, R. A.; Trent, R. B. (1988). "Supraorbital ridge and masticatory apparatus I: Primates". Human Evolution. 3 (5): 343–50. doi: 10.1007/BF02447216. S2CID  83923845.
  38. ^ Winkler, L. A. (1989). "Morphology and relationships of the orangutan fatty cheek pads". American Journal of Primatology. 17 (4): 305–19. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350170405. PMID  31964053. S2CID  83661029.
  39. ^ a b c Utami, S. S.; Goossens, B.; Bruford, M. W.; de Ruiter, J. R.; van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (2002). "Male bimaturism and reproductive success in Sumatran orangutans". Behavioral Ecology. 13 (5): 643–52. doi: 10.1093/beheco/13.5.643.
  40. ^ Rose, M. D. (1988). "Functional Anatomy of the Cheirdia". In Schwartz, Jeffrey (ed.). Orang-utan Biology. Oxford University Press. p. 301. ISBN  978-0-19-504371-6.
  41. ^ Oishi, M.; Ogihara, N.; Endo, H.; Ichihara, N.; Asari, M. (2009). "Dimensions of forelimb muscles in orangutans and chimpanzees". Journal of Anatomy. 215 (4): 373–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2009.01125.x. PMC  2766055. PMID  19619166.
  42. ^ Schwartz, Jeffrey (1987). The Red Ape: Orangutans and Human Origins. Westview Press. pp.  6–7. ISBN  978-0-8133-4064-7.
  43. ^ a b Reese, April (2 November 2017). "Newly discovered orangutan species is also the most endangered". Nature. 551 (7679): 151. Bibcode: 2017Natur.551..151R. doi: 10.1038/nature.2017.22934. PMID  29120449.
  44. ^ a b c d Galdikas, Birute M. F. (1988). "Orangutan Diet, Range, and Activity at Tanjung Puting, Central Borneo". International Journal of Primatology. 9 (1): 1–35. doi: 10.1007/BF02740195. S2CID  40513842.
  45. ^ Rodman, P. S. (1988). "Diversity and consistency in ecology and behavior". In Schwartz, J. H. (ed.). Orang-utan biology. Oxford University Press. pp. 31–51. ISBN  978-0195043716.
  46. ^ Foitová, Ivona; Huffman, Michael A.; Wisnu, Nurcahyo; Olšanský, Milan (2009). "Parasites and their impacts on orangutan health". In Wich, Serge A.; Atmoko, S. Suci Utami; Setia, Tatang Mitra; van Schaik, Carel P. (eds.). Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation. Oxford University Press. p.  166. ISBN  978-0199213276.
  47. ^ Morrogh-Bernard HC, Foitová I, Yeen Z, Wilkin P, de Martin R, Rárová L, Doležal K, Nurcahyo W, Olšanský M (2017). "Self-medication by orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus) using bioactive properties of Dracaena cantleyi". Scientific Reports. 7 (16653): 16653. Bibcode: 2017NatSR...716653M. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16621-w. PMC  5709421. PMID  29192145.
  48. ^ Zimmermann, N; Pirovino, M; Zingg, R; Clauss, M; Kaup, F. J.; Heistermann, M; Hatt, J. M.; Steinmetz, H. W. (2011). "Upper respiratory tract disease in captive orangutans (Pongo sp.): prevalence in 20 European zoos and predisposing factors". Journal of Medical Primatology. 40 (6): 365–375. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0684.2011.00490.x. PMID  21770970. S2CID  997385.
  49. ^ Rijksen, H. D. (December 1978). "A field study on Sumatran orang utans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii, Lesson 1827): Ecology, Behaviour and Conservation". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 53 (4): 493–94. doi: 10.1086/410942. JSTOR  2826733.
  50. ^ a b Teboekhorst, I.; Schürmann, C.; Sugardjito, J. (1990). "Residential status and seasonal movements of wild orang-utans in the Gunung Leuser Reserve (Sumatera, Indonesia)". Animal Behaviour. 39 (6): 1098–1109. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80782-1. S2CID  54322235.
  51. ^ a b Singleton, I.; van Schaik, C. P. (2002). "The Social Organisation of a population of Sumatran orang-utans". Folia Primatologica. 73 (1): 1–20. doi: 10.1159/000060415. PMID  12065937. S2CID  13557435.
  52. ^ Galdikas, B. M. F. (1984). "Adult female sociality among wild orangutans at Tanjung Puting Reserve". In Small, M. F. (ed.). Female Primates: Studies by Women Primatologists. Alan R. Liss. pp. 217–35. ISBN  978-0845134030.
  53. ^ Atmoko, S. Suci Utami; Singleton, Ian; van Noordwijk, Maria A.; van Schaik, Carel P.; Setia, Tatang Mitra (2009). "Male–male relationships in orangutans". In Wich, Serge A.; Atmoko, S. Suci Utami; Setia, Tatang Mitra; van Schaik, Carel P. (eds.). Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation. Oxford University Press. pp. 227–29. ISBN  978-0-19-921327-6.
  54. ^ van Noordwijk, Maria A.; Sauren, Simone E.B.; Nuzuar; Abulani, Ahbam; Morrogh-Bernard, Helen C.; Atmoko, S. Suci Utami; van Schaik, Carel P. (2009). "Development of Independence". In Wich, Serge A.; Atmoko, S. Suci Utami; Setia, Tatang Mitra; van Schaik, Carel P. (eds.). Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation. Oxford University Press. p. 199. ISBN  978-0199213276.
  55. ^ a b c Delgado, R. A. Jr.; van Schaik, C. P. (2000). "The behavioral ecology and conservation of the orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus): a tale of two islands". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 9 (1): 201–18. doi: 10.1002/1520-6505(2000)9:5<201::AID-EVAN2>3.0.CO;2-Y. S2CID  84778685.
  56. ^ a b van Schaik, C. P.; Preuschoft, S.; Watts, D. P. (2004). "Great ape social systems". In Russon, A. E.; Begun, D. R. (eds.). The Evolution of Thought: Evolutionary Origins of Great Ape Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. pp.  193–94. ISBN  978-0521039925.
  57. ^ van Schaik, C. P. (1999). "The socioecology of fission-fusion sociality in orangutans". Primates. 40 (1): 69–86. doi: 10.1007/BF02557703. PMID  23179533. S2CID  13366732.
  58. ^ Mitani, J. C.; Grether, G. F.; Rodman, P. S.; Priatna, D. (1991). "Associations among wild orang-utans: sociality, passive aggregations or chance". Animal Behaviour. 42 (1): 33–46. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80603-7. S2CID  40824300.
  59. ^ a b Munn, C.; Fernandez, M. (1997). "Infant development". In Sodaro, Carol (ed.). Orangutan Species Survival Plan Husbandry Manual. Chicago Zoological Park. pp. 59–66. OCLC  40349739.
  60. ^ "Orangutan call repertoires". Universität Zürich – Department of Anthropology. Archived from the original on 3 October 2020. Retrieved 23 April 2020.
  61. ^ Lameira, A. R.; et al. (2021). "Orangutan information broadcast via consonant-like and vowel-like calls breaches mathematical models of linguistic evolution". Biology Letters. 17 (9). doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2021.0302. PMC  8478518. PMID  34582737.
  62. ^ Knox, A; Markx, J; How, E; Azis, A; Hobaiter, C; an Veen, F. J. F; Morrogh-Bernard, H (2019). "Gesture use in communication between mothers and offspring in wild Orang-Utans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) from the Sabangau Peat-Swamp Forest, Borneo". International Journal of Primatology. 40 (3): 393–416. doi: 10.1007/s10764-019-00095-w. hdl: 10023/18054. S2CID  195329265.
  63. ^ Fox, E. A. (2002). "Female tactics to reduce sexual harassment in the Sumatran orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus abelii)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 52 (2): 93–101. doi: 10.1007/s00265-002-0495-x. S2CID  13583879.
  64. ^ a b c Knott, Cheryl Denise; Thompson, Melissa Emery; Stumpf, Rebecca M; McIntyre, Matthew H (2009). "Female reproductive strategies in orangutans, evidence for female choice and counterstrategies to infanticide in a species with frequent sexual coercion". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 277 (1678): 105–13. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1552. PMC  2842634. PMID  19812079.
  65. ^ Fox, Elizabeth A (2001). "Homosexual behavior in wild Sumatran orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii)". American Journal of Primatology. 55 (3): 177–81. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1051. PMID  11746281. S2CID  21561581.
  66. ^ Wich, Serge A.; de Vries, Hans; Ancrenaz, Marc; Perkins, Lori; Shumaker, Robert W.; Suzuki, Akira; van Schaik, Carel P. (2009). "Orangutan life history variation". In Wich, Serge A.; Atmoko, S. Suci Utami; Setia, Tatang Mitra; van Schaik, Carel P. (eds.). Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation. Oxford University Press. pp. 67–68. ISBN  978-0199213276.
  67. ^ Goossens, B; Mohd, D; Kapar; Kahar, S (2011). "First Sighting of Bornean Orangutan Twins in the Wild". Asian Primates Journal. 2 (1): 10–12.
  68. ^ Beaudrot, LH; Kahlenberg, SM; Marshall, AJ (2009). "Why male orangutans do not kill infants". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 63 (11): 1549–62. doi: 10.1007/s00265-009-0827-1. PMC  2728907. PMID  19701484.
  69. ^ Scott, A. M.; Knott, C. D.; Susanto, T. W. (2019). "Are Male Orangutans a Threat to Infants? Evidence of Mother–Offspring Counterstrategies to Infanticide in Bornean Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii)". International Journal of Primatology. 44 (3): 435–55. doi: 10.1007/s10764-019-00097-8. hdl: 2144/39274. S2CID  198190605.
  70. ^ Smith, Tanya M.; Austin, Christine; Hinde, Katie; Vogel, Erin R.; Arora, Manish (2017). "Cyclical nursing patterns in wild orangutans". Evolutionary Biology. 3 (5): e1601517. Bibcode: 2017SciA....3E1517S. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1601517. PMC  5435413. PMID  28560319.
  71. ^ a b Didik, Prasetyo; Ancrenaz, Marc; Morrogh-Bernard, Helen C.; Atmoko, S. Suci Utami; Wich, Serge A.; van Schaik, Carel P. (2009). "Nest building in orangutans". In Wich, Serge A.; Atmoko, S. Suci Utami; Setia, Tatang Mitra; van Schaik, Carel P. (eds.). Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation. Oxford University Press. pp. 270–75. ISBN  978-0-19-921327-6. Archived from the original on 24 April 2016. Retrieved 16 December 2015.
  72. ^ a b Lameira, A. R.; Hardus, M. E.; Shumaker, R. W.; Wich, S. A.; Menken, S. B. J. (2015). "Speech-Like Rhythm in a Voiced and Voiceless Orangutan Call". PLOS ONE. 10 (1): e116136. Bibcode: 2015PLoSO..10k6136L. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0116136. PMC  4287529. PMID  25569211.
  73. ^ Deaner, R. O.; van Schaik, C. P.; Johnson, V. (2006). "Do some taxa have better domain-general cognition than others? A meta-analysis of nonhuman primate studies". Evolutionary Psychology. 4: 149–96. doi: 10.1177/147470490600400114. S2CID  16702785. Archived from the original on 19 August 2021. Retrieved 19 June 2020.
  74. ^ de Blois, S. T.; Novak, M. A.; Bond, M. (1998). "Object Permanence in Orangutans (Pongo Pygmaeus) and Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri Sciureus)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 112 (2): 137–52. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.137. PMID  9642783.
  75. ^ Turner, Dorie (12 April 2007). "Orangutans play video games (for research) at Georgia zoo". USA Today. Archived from the original on 2 July 2007. Retrieved 12 April 2007.
  76. ^ Dufour, V.; Pelé, M.; Neumann, M.; Thierry, B.; Call, J. (2008). "Calculated reciprocity after all: computation behind token transfers in orang-utans". Biology Letters. 5 (2): 172–75. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0644. PMC  2665816. PMID  19126529.
  77. ^ Chalmeau, Raphaël; Lardeux, Karine; Brandibas, Pierre; Gallo, Alain (1997). "Cooperative problem solving by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus)". International Journal of Primatology. 18 (1): 23–32. doi: 10.1023/A:1026337006136. S2CID  44204663.
  78. ^ Suárez, S. D.; Gallup, G. G. (1981). "Self-recognition in chimpanzees and orangutans, but not gorillas". Journal of Human Evolution. 10 (2): 175–88. doi: 10.1016/s0047-2484(81)80016-4.
  79. ^ Robert, S. (1986). "Ontogeny of mirror behavior in two species of great apes". American Journal of Primatology. 10 (2): 109–17. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350100202. PMID  31979488. S2CID  85330986.
  80. ^ van Schaik, Carel P.; Damerius, L.; Isler, K. (2013). "Wild Orangutan Males Plan and Communicate Their Travel Direction One Day in Advance". PLoS One. 8 (9): e74896. Bibcode: 2013PLoSO...874896V. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074896. PMC  3770631. PMID  24040357.
  81. ^ Lameira, Adriano R.; Call, Josep (2018). "Time-space–displaced responses in the orangutan vocal system". Science Advances. 4 (11): eaau3401. Bibcode: 2018SciA....4.3401L. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aau3401. PMC  6235548. PMID  30443595.
  82. ^ Ross, Marina Davila; Owren, Michael J; Zimmermann, Elke (2009). "Reconstructing the Evolution of Laughter in Great Apes and Humans". Current Biology. 19 (13): 1106–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.028. PMID  19500987. S2CID  17892549.
  83. ^ Lameira, A. R.; Shumaker, R. W. (2019). "Orangutans show active voicing through a membranophone". Scientific Reports. 9 (1): 12289. Bibcode: 2019NatSR...912289L. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-48760-7. PMC  6707206. PMID  31444387.
  84. ^ Wich, S. A.; Swartz, K. B.; Hardus, M. E.; Lameira, A. R.; Stromberg, E.; Shumaker, R. W. (2008). "A case of spontaneous acquisition of a human sound by an orangutan". Primates. 50 (1): 56–64. doi: 10.1007/s10329-008-0117-y. PMID  19052691. S2CID  708682.
  85. ^ Galdikas, B. M. F. (1982). "Orang-Utan tool use at Tanjung Putting Reserve, Central Indonesian Borneo (Kalimantan Tengah)". Journal of Human Evolution. 10: 19–33. doi: 10.1016/S0047-2484(82)80028-6.
  86. ^ Fox, E. A.; Sitompul, A. F.; van Schaik, C. P. (1999). "Intelligent tool use in wild Sumatran orangutans". In Parker, S; Mitchell, R. W.; Miles, H. L. (eds.). The Mentality of Gorillas and Orangutans. Cambridge University Press. pp. 99–116. ISBN  978-0-521-03193-6.
  87. ^ van Schaik, C. P.; Fox, E. A.; Sitompul, A. F. (1996). "Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans – implications or human evolution". Naturwissenschaften. 83 (4): 186–88. Bibcode: 1996NW.....83..186V. doi: 10.1007/BF01143062. PMID  8643126. S2CID  27180148.
  88. ^ O'Malley, R. C.; McGrew, W. C. (2000). "Oral tool use by captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus)". Folia Primatologica. 71 (5): 334–41. doi: 10.1159/000021756. PMID  11093037. S2CID  19354930.
  89. ^ Russon, A. E.; Compost, A.; Kuncoro, P.; Ferisa, A. (2014). "Orangutan Fish Eating, Primate Aquatic Fauna Eating, and Their Implications for the Origins of Ancestral Hominin Fish Eating". Journal of Human Evolution. 77: 50–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.06.007. PMID  25038033.
  90. ^ Bower, B. (18 April 2011). "Orangutans use simple tools to catch fish". Wired. Archived from the original on 8 July 2013. Retrieved 5 August 2013.
  91. ^ Mulcahy, N. J. (2018). "An Orangutan Hangs Up a Tool for Future Use". Scientific Reports. 8 (1): 1–6. Bibcode: 2018NatSR...812900M. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-31331-7. PMC  6110832. PMID  30150738.
  92. ^ van Casteren, A.; Sellers, W. I.; Thorpe, S. K. S.; Coward, S.; Crompton, R. H.; Myatt, J. P.; Ennos, A. R. (2012). "Nest-building orangutans demonstrate engineering know-how to produce safe, comfortable beds". PNAS. 109 (18): 6873–77. Bibcode: 2012PNAS..109.6873V. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200902109. PMC  3344992. PMID  22509022.
  93. ^ a b van Schaik, Carel P.; Knott, Cheryl D. (2001). "Geographic variation in tool use onNeesia fruits in orangutans". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 114 (4): 331–342. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1045. PMID  11275962.
  94. ^ van Schaik, CP; Van Noordwijk, MA; Wich, SA. (2006). "Innovation in wild Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii)". Behaviour. 143 (7): 839–76. doi: 10.1163/156853906778017944.
  95. ^ Russon, AE; Handayani, DP; Kuncoro, P; Ferisa, A. (2007). "Orangutan leaf-carrying for nest-building: toward unraveling cultural processes". Animal Cognition. 10 (2): 189–202. doi: 10.1007/s10071-006-0058-z. PMID  17160669. S2CID  6875716.
  96. ^ Hardus, M. E.; Lameira, A. R.; van Schaik, C. P.; Wich, S. A. (2009). "Tool use in wild orang-utans modifies sound production: a functionally deceptive innovation?". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 276 (1673): 3689–94. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1027. PMC  2817314. PMID  19656794.
  97. ^ van Schaik, C. P.; Ancrenaz, M.; Borgen, G.; Galdikas, B.; Knott, C. D.; Singleton, I.; Suzuki, A.; Utami, S. S.; Merrill, M.; et al. (2003). "Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture". Science. 299 (5603): 102–05. Bibcode: 2003Sci...299..102V. doi: 10.1126/science.1078004. PMID  12511649. S2CID  25139547.
  98. ^ Glendinning, L. (26 June 2008). "Spanish parliament approves 'human rights' for apes". The Guardian. Archived from the original on 26 August 2009. Retrieved 10 November 2008.
  99. ^ Singer, P. (18 July 2008). "Of great apes and men". The Guardian. Archived from the original on 13 November 2012. Retrieved 10 November 2008.
  100. ^ "Sandra".
  101. ^ Giménez, Emiliano (23 December 2014). "Argentine orangutan granted unprecedented legal rights". CNN. Archived from the original on 3 April 2021. Retrieved 15 June 2020.
  102. ^ Meijer, Miriam Claude (2014). Race and Aesthetics in the Anthropology of Petrus Camper (1722–1789) (Studies in the History of Ideas in the Low Countries). Rodopi. p. 42. ISBN  978-9042004344.
  103. ^ de Waal, Frans (January 1995). "The Loneliest of Apes". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 4 May 2013. Retrieved 26 February 2012.
  104. ^ Oaks, Elizabeth H. (2007). Encyclopedia of World Scientists: From Around the World. Facts on File. p. 260. ISBN  978-0816041305.
  105. ^ McDowell, Robin (18 January 2009). "Palm oil frenzy threatens to wipe out orangutans". Associated Press. Archived from the original on 20 January 2009. Retrieved 18 January 2009.
  106. ^ MacClancy, J.; Fuentes, A. (2010). Centralizing Fieldwork: Critical Perspectives from Primatology, Biological, and Social Anthropology. Berghahn Books. pp. 6–7. ISBN  978-1-84545-690-0. Archived from the original on 30 April 2016. Retrieved 16 December 2015.
  107. ^ a b van Wyhe, John; Kjærgaard, Peter C. (1 June 2015). "Going the whole orang: Darwin, Wallace and the natural history of orangutans". Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences. 51: 53–63. doi: 10.1016/j.shpsc.2015.02.006. ISSN  1369-8486. PMID  25861859.
  108. ^ Zimmer, Carl (21 April 2015). "When Darwin Met Another Ape". National Geographic Society. Archived from the original on 7 June 2020. Retrieved 24 February 2020.
  109. ^ Robeson, Dave (1935). Al G. Barnes, Master Showman, as told by Al G. Barnes. Caldwell, Idaho: The Caxton Printers, Ltd. pp. 102–103. LCCN  35012032. OCLC  598387.
  110. ^ "Animal Actors". PETA. Archived from the original on 3 March 2010. Retrieved 28 January 2011.
  111. ^ Middleton, Duncan (7 August 2016). "The missing lynx and five other animal escapees including Ken Allen the Orangutan". BBC Newsbeat. Archived from the original on 25 December 2018. Retrieved 25 December 2018.
  112. ^ Wrangham, Richard W; Peterson, Dale (1996). Demonic Males: Apes and the Origins of Human Violence. Houghton Mifflin. p.  137. ISBN  978-0-395-69001-7.
  113. ^ van Schaik, Carel (2004). Among Orangutans: Red Apes and the Rise of Human Culture. Harvard University Press. p.  88. ISBN  9780674015777.
  114. ^ Buck, Frank (2000). Bring 'em back alive : the best of Frank Buck. Steven Lehrer. Lubbock, Tex.: Texas Tech University Press. p. 37. ISBN  0-89672-430-1. OCLC  43207125.
  115. ^ a b c Singleton, I.; Wich, S. A.; Griffiths, M. (2008). "Pongo abelii". IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2008. Retrieved 28 January 2011.
  116. ^ a b c d Ancrenaz, M.; Gumal, M.; Marshall, A. J.; Meijaard, E.; Wich, S. A.; Husson, S. (2016). "Pongo pygmaeus". IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2016: e.T17975A17966347. doi: 10.2305/IUCN.UK.2016-1.RLTS.T17975A17966347.en.
  117. ^ a b Nowak, Matthew G.; Rianti, Puji; Wich, Serge A.; Meijaard, Erik; Fredriksson, Gabriella (2017). "Pongo tapanuliensis". IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2017.3. 2017: e.T120588639A120588662. doi: 10.2305/IUCN.UK.2017-3.RLTS.T120588639A120588662.en.
  118. ^ Neme, Laurel (11 October 2014). "Endangered Orangutans Gain From Eco-Friendly Shifts in Palm Oil Market". National Geographic Society. Archived from the original on 29 June 2020. Retrieved 1 July 2020.
  119. ^ "On the edge of extinction". Sumatran Orangutan Society. Archived from the original on 8 May 2020. Retrieved 1 July 2020.
  120. ^ Cheyne, S. M.; Thompson, C. J.; Phillips, A. C.; Hill, R. M.; Limin, S. H. (2007). "Density and population estimate of gibbons (Hylobates albibarbis) in the Sabangau catchment, Central Kalimantan, Indonesia". Primates. 49 (1): 50–56. doi: 10.1007/s10329-007-0063-0. PMID  17899314. S2CID  1792717.
  121. ^ Hays, Jeffrey. "Endangered Orangutans: Fires, Poaching and Palm Oil: Facts and Details". factsanddetails.com. Archived from the original on 12 May 2019. Retrieved 12 May 2019.
  122. ^ Than, Ker (16 November 2011). "Hundreds of Orangutans Killed Annually for Meat". National Geographic Society. Archived from the original on 3 October 2020. Retrieved 11 May 2020.
  123. ^ "Stop orangutan skull trade". Antara News. 7 September 2011. Archived from the original on 3 January 2012. Retrieved 16 January 2012.
  124. ^ Meijaard, E.; Buchori, B.; Hadiprakarsa, Y.; Utami-Atmoko, S. S.; et al. (2011). "Quantifying Killing of Orangutans and Human-Orangutan Conflict in Kalimantan, Indonesia". PLOS ONE. 6 (11): e27491. Bibcode: 2011PLoSO...627491M. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027491. PMC  3214049. PMID  22096582.
  125. ^ Davis, J. T.; Mengersen, K.; Abram, N. K.; Ancrenaz, M.; Wells, J. A.; Meijaard, E. (2013). "It's Not Just Conflict That Motivates Killing of Orangutans". PLOS ONE. 8 (10): e75373. Bibcode: 2013PLoSO...875373D. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075373. PMC  3793980. PMID  24130707.
  126. ^ Jacobson, Phillip (29 March 2017). "The military family that kept a pet orangutan in Indonesia". Mongabay. Archived from the original on 14 June 2020. Retrieved 11 May 2020.
  127. ^ Wich, S A; Meijaard, E; Marshall, A J; Husson, S; et al. (2002). "Distribution and conservation status of the orang-utan (Pongo spp.) on Borneo and Sumatra: how many remain?". Oryx. 42 (3): 329–39. doi: 10.5167/uzh-3914.
  128. ^ Wich, Serge A.; Singleton, Ian; Nowak, Matthew G.; Utami Atmoko, Sri Suci; Nisam, Gonda; Arif, Sugesti Mhd.; Putra, Rudi H.; Ardi, Rio; Fredriksson, Gabriella; Usher, Graham; Gaveau, David L. A.; Kühl, Hjalmar S. (2016). "Land-cover changes predict steep declines for the Sumatran orangutan (Pongo abelii)". Science Advances. 2 (3): e1500789. Bibcode: 2016SciA....2E0789W. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1500789. PMC  4783118. PMID  26973868.
  129. ^ Voigt, M; Wich, S. A.; Ancrenaz, M; et al. (2018). "Global demand for natural resources eliminated more than 100,000 Bornean Orangutans". Current Biology. 28 (5): P761–769.E5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.01.053. PMID  29456144. S2CID  3756682.
  130. ^ Nater, Alexander; Mattle-Greminger, Maja P.; Nurcahyo, Anton; Nowak, Matthew G.; Manuel, Marc de; Desai, Tariq; Groves, Colin; Pybus, Marc; Sonay, Tugce Bilgin (2 November 2017). "Morphometric, Behavioral, and Genomic Evidence for a New Orangutan Species". Current Biology. 27 (22): 3487–3498.e10. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.047. hdl: 10230/34400. ISSN  0960-9822. PMID  29103940.
  131. ^ "Willie Smits". Masarang Foundation. Archived from the original on 28 September 2020. Retrieved 11 May 2020.
  132. ^ "10 Years: Nyaru Menteng 1999–2009" (PDF). Orangutan Protection Foundation. Archived from the original (PDF) on 15 February 2010.
  133. ^ "Ny projektledare på Nyaru Menteng". Save the Orangutan. 26 January 2012. Archived from the original on 10 January 2014. Retrieved 2 April 2012.
  134. ^ "Orangutan shaved, made up and prostituted to men for six years". The Week. 28 November 2018. Archived from the original on 3 October 2020. Retrieved 29 April 2020.
  135. ^ Brady, Heather (18 May 2017). "Extremely Rare Albino Orangutan Found in Indonesia". National Geographic Society. Archived from the original on 17 February 2020. Retrieved 29 April 2020.
  136. ^ "Viewing Orangutans in the Wild". Sustainable Travel International. Archived from the original on 3 October 2020. Retrieved 10 May 2020.
  137. ^ Palmer, Alexandra (2020). Ethical Debates in Orangutan Conservation. Taylor & Francis. p. 127. ISBN  978-0429576638.
  138. ^ Martinelli, D. (2010). A Critical Companion to Zoosemiotics: People, Paths, Ideas. Springer. pp. 218–19. ISBN  978-90-481-9248-9. Archived from the original on 30 April 2016. Retrieved 16 December 2015.
  139. ^ "Tax Deductible Organisations (Register of Environmental Organisations)". Australian Department of the Environment and Water Resources. Archived from the original on 10 February 2007. Retrieved 16 January 2014.
  140. ^ Butler, R. A. (20 August 2009). "Rehabilitation not enough to solve orangutan crisis in Indonesia". Mongabay. Archived from the original on 5 January 2012. Retrieved 26 March 2012.
  141. ^ Marusiak, J. (28 June 2011). "New deal for orangutans in Kalimantan". Eco-Business. Retrieved 3 April 2012.

External links