From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This article is about the sound system of the Navajo language. The phonology of Navajo is intimately connected to its morphology. For example, the entire range of contrastive consonants is found only at the beginning of word stems. In stem-final position and in prefixes, the number of contrasts is drastically reduced. Similarly, vowel contrasts (including their prosodic combinatory possibilities) found outside of the stem are significantly neutralized. For details about the morphology of Navajo, see Navajo grammar.

Like most Athabascan languages, Navajo is coronal heavy, having many phonological contrasts at coronal places of articulation and less at other places. Also typical of the family, Navajo has a limited number of labial sounds, both in terms of its phonemic inventory and in their occurrence in actual lexical items and displays of consonant harmony.

Consonants

The consonant phonemes of Navajo are listed below.

Bilabial Alveolar Palato-
alveolar
Palatal Velar Glottal
plain lateral affricate plain lab.
Nasal plain m n
glottalized ( ) ( )
Stop unaspirated p t ts k ʔ
aspirated tɬʰ tsʰ tʃʰ kʷʰ
ejective tɬʼ tsʼ tʃʼ
Continuant fortis s ɬ ʃ x
lenis z l ʒ j ɣ ɣʷ
Glide plain
glottalized ( ) ( )

Phonetics

All consonants are long, compared to English: with plain stops the hold is longer, with aspirated stops the aspiration is longer, and with affricates the frication is longer. The voice onset time of the aspirated and ejective stops is twice as long as that found in most non-Athabaskan languages. Young & Morgan (1987) described Navajo consonants as "doubled" between vowels, but in fact they are equally long in all positions. [1]

Stops and affricates

All stops and affricates, except for the bilabial and glottal, have a three-way laryngeal contrast between unaspirated, aspirated, and ejective. The labial /m/ is found in only a few words. Most of the contrasts in the inventory lie within coronal territory at the alveolar and palatoalveolar places of articulation.

The aspirated stops /tʰ, kʰ/ (orthographic t, k) are typically aspirated with velar frication [tx, kx] (they are phonetically affricateshomorganic in the case of [kx], heterorganic in the case of [tx]). [2] The velar aspiration is also found on a labialized velar [kxʷ] (orthographic kw). There is variation within Navajo, however, in this respect: some dialects lack strong velar frication, having instead a period of aspiration. [3] [4]

Similarly, the unaspirated velar /k/ (orthographic g) is realized with optional voiced velar frication following the stop burst: [k] ~ [kɣ].[ citation needed] The unaspirated lateral /tɬ/ (orthographic dl) typically has a voiced lateral release, [tˡ], of a duration comparable to the release of the /k/ and much shorter than the unaspirated fricatives /ts/, /tʃ/. However, the aspirated and ejective laterals are true affricates.

While the aspiration of stops is markedly long compared to most other languages, the aspiration of the affricates is quite short: the main feature distinguishing /ts/, /tʃ/ from /tsʰ/, /tʃʰ/ is that the frication is half again as long in the latter: [tsˑʰ], [tʃʰˑ]. /tɬʰ/ is similarly long, [tɬˑʰ]. The ejectives /tsʼ/, /tɬʼ/, /tʃʼ/, on the other hand, have short frication, presumably due to the lack of pulmonic airflow. There is a period of near silence before the glottalized onset of the vowel. In /tɬʼ/ there may be a double glottal release, or a creaky onset to the vowel not found in the other ejective affricates.

Continuants

Navajo voiceless continuants are realized as fricatives. They are typically noisier than the fricatives that occur in English. The palato-alveolars /ʃ, ʒ/ are not labialized unlike English and other European languages. [5]

Navajo also does not have consistent phonetic voicing in the "voiced" continuant members. Although /z, l, ʒ, ɣ/ are described as voiced in impressionist descriptions, [6] data from spectrograms shows that they may be partially devoiced during the constriction. In stem-initial position, /l/ tends to be fully voiced, /ʒ/ has a slight tendency to be voiceless near the offset, /z/ is often mostly voiceless with phonetic voicing only at the onset, /ɣ/ is also only partially voiced with voicing at onset. A more consistent acoustic correlate of the "voicing" is the duration of the consonant: "voiceless" consonants have longer durations than "voiced" consonants. Based on this, McDonough (2003) argues that the distinction is better captured with the notion of a fortis/lenis contrast. A further characteristic of voicing in Navajo is that it is marginally contrastive (see the voicing assimilation section).

Navajo lacks a clear distinction between phonetic fricatives and approximants. Although the pair [ɬ]~[l] has been described as a fricative and an approximant, respectively, the lack of a consistent contrast between the two phonetic categories and a similar patterning with other fricative pairs suggests that they are better described as continuants. Additionally, observations have been made about the less fricative-like nature of [ɣ, ɣʷ] and the more fricative-like nature of [j].

Sonorants

A more abstract analysis of Navajo posits two different /j/ phonemes (see below for elaboration).

The glottalized sonorants are the result of d-effect on the non-glottalized counterparts. A strict structuralist analysis, such as that of Hoijer (1945a) and Sapir & Hoijer (1967), considers them phonemic.

Glottal(ized) consonants

Consonants involving a glottal closure – the glottal stop, ejective stops, and the glottalized sonorants – may have optional creaky voice on voiced sounds adjacent to the glottal gesture. Glottal stops may also be realized entirely as creaky voice instead of single glottal closure. [7] Ejectives in Navajo differ from the ejectives in many other languages in that the glottal closure is not released near-simultaneously with the release of the oral closure (as is common in other languages) – it is held for a significant amount of time following oral release. The glottalized sonorants /mˀ, nˀ/ are articulated with a glottal stop preceding the oral closure with optional creaky voice during the oral closure: [ʔm ʔm̰, ʔn ʔn̰].

Labialized consonants

Consonants /kʰʷ, xʷ, ɣʷ, hʷ/ are predictable variants that occur before the rounded oral vowel /o/. However, these sounds also occur before the vowels /i, e, a/ where they contrast with their non-labialized counterparts /kʰ, x, ɣ, h/.

Velar /ɣ/, palatal /j/

The phonological contrast between the velar obstruent /ɣ/ and the palatal glide /j/ is neutralized in certain contexts. However, in these contexts, they may often be distinguished from each other by their different phonological patterning.

Before the rounded /o/, /ɣ/ is phonetically strongly labialized as [ɣʷ]; elsewhere, it lacks the labialization. As noted above, the lenis continuants like /ɣ/ are often very weak fricatives somewhere between a typical fricative constriction (e.g. [ɣ]) and a more open approximant constriction (e.g. [ɰ]) – this will be symbolized here as [ɰ̝]. Hoijer (1945a) describes the [ɰ̝ʷ] realization as being similar to English [w] but differing in having slight frication at the beginning of the articulation. The realization before /a/ varies between an approximant [ɰ] and a weakly fricated approximant [ɰ̝]. [8] The following verb stem [9] has different velar allophones of the stem-initial consonant:

Word Underlying Phonetic ! Gloss
-ghash /ɣàʃ/ [ɰ̝àʃ] 'make bubbling noise' (iterative, continuative)
-wosh /ɣòʃ/ [ɰ̝ʷòʃ] 'make bubbling noise' (iterative, repetitive)

The palatal glide /j/ is also phonetically between an approximant [j] and a fricative [ʝ]. Hoijer (1945a) compares it to English [j] with a "slight but audible 'rubbiness' or frication."

The contrast between velar /ɣ/ and palatal /j/ is found before both back vowels /a, o/ as the following contrasts demonstrate:

Word Underlying Phonetic Gloss
contrast before /a/ bighaaʼ /pìɣàːʔ/ [pɪ̀ɰ̝àːʔ] 'its fur'
biyaaʼ /pìjàːʔ/ [pɪ̀j˔àːʔ] 'its lice'
contrast before /o/ biwol /pìɣòl/ [pɪ̀ɰ̝ʷòl] 'its marrow'
biyol /pìjòl/ [pɪ̀j˔òl] 'its breath'

Before the front vowels /i, e/, however, the contrast between /ɣ/ and /j/ is neutralized to a palatal articulation much like the weakly fricative [j˔] realization of /j/ that occurs before back vowels. However, the underlying consonant can be ascertained in verb stems and noun stems via their different realizations in a voiceless (i.e. fortis) context. The underlying velar surfaces as a voiceless palatal fricative [ç] in these environments:

Fortis context Lenis context
Word Phonetic Gloss Word Phonetic Gloss
hééł [çéːɬ] 'bundle' biyéél [pɪ̀j˔éːl] 'her bundle'
yishhizh [j˔ɪ̀ʃçɪ̀ʒ] 'I pick (corn)' yiyizh [j˔ɪ̀j˔ɪ̀ʒ] 'she picks (corn)'

The stem-initial velar of the noun stem /xéːɬ/ has a voiceless fortis realization of [ç] (as [çéːɬ]) when word-initial. When intervocalic, it is realized as lenis [j˔] (as [-j˔éːl]). Likewise, the underlying velar of the verb stem /xɪ̀ʒ/ is a voiceless [ç] after the preceding voiceless [ʃ] and lenis [j˔] when intervocalic. Thus, the alternation of ~ j˔] in the two contexts is indicative of an underlying velar consonant. Similarly, before the back vowels, the velar continuant has the alternations [x ~ ɰ̝] and [xʷ ~ ɰ̝ʷ], as shown in the examples below:

Fortis context Lenis context
Word Phonetic Gloss Word Phonetic Gloss
before /a/ haníłháásh [hànɪ́ɬxáːʃ] 'you make it boil' hanílgháásh [hànɪ́lɰ̝áːʃ] 'it comes to a boil'
before /o/ ałhosh [ʔàɬxʷòʃ] 'he's sleeping' áhodilwosh [ʔáhòtɪ̀lɰ̝ʷòʃ] 'he's pretending to be asleep'

An underlying palatal /j/ can be determined by alternations which differ from the velar alternations. However, /j/ has two different alternation patterns, which have led to the positing of two distinct phonemes. Incidentally, the two different phonemes are also connected to two different reconstructed consonants in Proto-Athabascan. One of these /j/ phonemes is considered an obstruent as it has a fricative realization of [s] in fortis contexts. It is often symbolized as a palatalized (or front velar) fricative /ɣ̑/ (in Americanist phonetic notation) and is a reflex of Proto-Athabascan *. It may be considered coronal because of its coronal voiceless allophone.

Fortis context Lenis context
Word Phonetic Gloss Word Phonetic Gloss
before /i/ sin [sɪ̀n] 'song' biyiin [pɪ̀j˔ìːn] 'her song'
before /a/ honissą́ [hònɪ̀sːã́] 'I'm wise' hóyą́ [hój˔ã́] 'she's wise'
before /o/ hanisóód [hànɪ̀sːóːt] 'I drive them out' hainiyóód [hàɪ̀nɪ̀j˔óːt] 'she drives them out'

In the above examples, the fortis realization is [s] in the stems [sɪ̀n], [-sã́], [-sóːt], while the lenis realization is the glide [j˔] in the corresponding [-j˔ɪ̀n], [-j˔ã́], [-j˔óːt]. Since the fortis reflex of this phoneme is [s], there is also a neutralization between this /j/ phoneme and the alveolar /s/ phoneme. The alveolar phoneme has a [s ~ z] alternation in fortis-lenis contexts:

Fortis context Lenis context
Word Phonetic Gloss Word Phonetic Gloss
séí [séɪ́] 'sand' bizéí [pɪ̀zéɪ́] 'her sand'

Thus, the different alternations also distinguish between underlying /j/ and underlying /s/.

The other underlying (or morphophonemic) palatal /j/ is considered a sonorant and has an invariant [j˔] realization in both fortis (voiceless) and lenis (voiced) contexts. This phoneme is relatively rare, occurring in only a few morphemes. It is a reflex of Proto-Athabascan *y (as symbolized in Americanist notation). Two examples are below:

Fortis context Lenis context
Word Phonetic Gloss Word Phonetic Gloss
before /a/ yaaʼ [j˔àːʔ] 'louse' shiyaʼ [ʃɪ̀j˔àʔ] 'my louse'
before /o/ honishyóí [hònɪ̀ʃj˔óɪ́] 'I'm energetic' honíyóí [hònɪ́j˔óɪ́] 'you're energetic'

A further distinction between the different phonemes is found in the context of d-effect.

The varying contextual realizations of these three underlying segments are summarized in the following table:

Underlying segment Lenis Fortis D-effect
before /a/ before /o/ before /i, e/
/ɣ/ ɰ̝ ɰ̝ʷ x k
/j/
< Proto-Ath. *
s ts
/j/
< Proto-Ath. *y
jˀ˔

Voicing assimilation

The voiced continuants /z, l, ʒ, ɰ̝/ at the beginning of stems vary with their voiceless counterparts /s, ɬ, ʃ, x/, respectively. The voiceless variants occur when preceded by voiceless consonants, such as /s, ɬ, ʃ, h/ while the voiced variants occur between voiced sounds (typically intervocalically). For example, the verb stems meaning 'spit it out', 'be burning', 'spit', and 'be ticklish' have the following forms with alternating voiced and voiceless stem-initial consonants:

Phonetic forms Orthographic forms Gloss
[zóːh ~ sóːh] -zóóh ~ -sóóh 'spit it out'
[lɪ̀t ~ ɬɪ̀t] -lid ~ -łid 'be burning'
[ʒàh ~ ʃàh] -zhah ~ -shah 'spit'
[ɰ̝ʷòʒ ~ xʷòʒ] -wozh ~ -hozh 'be ticklish'

Since the voicing is predictable, it can be represented more abstractly as an underlying consonant that is underspecified with respect to voicing. These archiphonemes can be indicated with the capital letters /Z, L, Ʒ, Ɣ/. [10] The variant voicing of the stem-initial consonant can be found in the context of subject person prefixes which are added to the verb stem:

Phonetic form Orthographic form Underlying segments [11] Gloss
[hàɪ̀tɪ̀zóːh] haidizóóh hàìtì-∅-Zóːh 'he spits it out'
[hàtɪ̀sóːh] hadisóóh hàtì-ʃ-Zóːh 'I spit it out'
[hàtòhsóːh] hadohsóóh hàtì-oh-Zóːh 'you two spit it out'
[tɪ̀lɪ̀t] dilid tì-∅-Lìt 'he's burning'
[tɪ̀ʃɬɪ̀t] dishłid tì-ʃ-Lìt 'I'm burning'
[tòhɬɪ̀t] dohłid tì-oh-Lìt 'you two are burning'
[tɪ̀ʒàh] dizhah tì-∅-Ʒàh 'he spits'
[tɪ̀ʃàh] dishah tì-ʃ-Ʒàh 'I spit'
[tòhʃàh] dohshah tì-oh-Ʒàh 'you two spit'
[jɪ̀ɰ̝ʷòʒ] yiwozh ∅-Ɣòʒ 'he's ticklish'
[jɪ̀ʃxʷòʒ] yishhozh ∅-ʃ-Ɣòʒ 'I'm ticklish'
[wòhxʷòʒ] wohhozh ∅-oh-Ɣòʒ 'you two are ticklish'

As the above examples show, the stem-initial consonant is voiced when intervocalic [12] and voiceless when it is preceded by a voiceless /ʃ-/ sh- first person singular subject prefix [13] or a voiceless [h] in the /oh-/ oh- two person dual subject prefix.

Another example of contextual voicing of verb-stem-initial consonants occurs when a voiceless /-ɬ-/ -ł- classifier prefix occurs before the stem as in the following:

Phonetic form Orthographic form Underlying segments Gloss
[tìːlzáːs] diilzáás tì-Vt-ɬ-Záːs 'we two dribble it along'
[jɪ̀tɪ̀sáːs] yidisáás jìtì-ɬ-Záːs 'he dribbles it along'
[tɪ̀sáːs] disáás tì-ʃ-ɬ-Záːs 'I dribble it along'
[tòhsáːs] dohsáás tì-oh-ɬ-Záːs 'you two dribble it along'

In the verb-form [tìːlzáːs] diilzáás ('we two dribble it along'), the /Z/ occurs between a voiced [l] and the voiced stem vowel [áː]. Thus it is realized as a voiced [z]. Here the /-ɬ-/ classifier is voiced due to the d-effect of the preceding /Vt-/ first person dual subject prefix. In the other verb-forms, the stem-initial /Z/ is preceded by voiceless /-ɬ-/ classifier which results in a voiceless realization of [s]. In the surface verb-forms, the underlying /-ɬ-/ classifier is not pronounced due to a phonotactic restriction on consonant clusters.

The initial consonant of noun stems also display contextual voicing:

Phonetic form Orthographic form Underlying segments Gloss
[sàːt] saad sàːt 'language'
[pɪ̀zàːt] bizaad pì-sàːt 'his language'
[ɬɪ̀t] łid ɬìt 'smoke'
[pɪ̀lɪ̀t] bilid pì-ɬìt 'his smoke'
[ʃàːʒ] shaazh ʃàːʒ 'callous'
[pɪ̀ʒàːʒ] bizhaazh pì-ʃàːʒ "his callous"
[xʷòʃ] hosh xòʃ "cactus"
[pɪ̀ɰ̝ʷòʃ] biwosh pì-xòʃ "his cactus"

Here an intervocalic context is created by inflecting the nouns saad, łid, shaazh, hosh with a [pɪ̀-] bi- third person prefix which ends in a vowel. In this context, the stem-initial consonant is voiced. When these nouns lack a prefix (in which case the stem-initial consonant is word-initial), the realization is voiceless.

However, in some noun stems, the stem-initial continuant does not voice when intervocalic: [ʔàʃĩ̀ːh] ashįįh ('salt').

Dorsal place assimilation

The dorsal consonants /k, kʰ, kʼ, x, ɣ/ (written g, k, , h, gh) have contextual phonetic variants (i.e. allophones) varying along place of articulation that depend on the following vowel environment. They are realized as palatals before the front vowels i and e and as velars before the back vowels a and o. Additionally, they are labialized before the rounded back vowel o. This likewise happens with the velar frication of the aspirated /tʰ/.

Phoneme Allophones
Palatal Velar Labial
k [c(ʝ)] [k(ɣ)] [k(ɣ)ʷ]
[cç] [kx] [kxʷ]
[cʼ] [kʼ] [kʼʷ]
x [ç] [x] [xʷ]
ɣ [j˔] [ɰ̝] [ɰ̝ʷ]
[tç] [tx] [txʷ]

Coronal harmony

Navajo has coronal sibilant consonant harmony. Alveolar sibilants in prefixes assimilate to post-alveolar sibilants in stems, and post-alveolar prefixal sibilants assimilate to alveolar stem sibilants. For example, the si- stative perfective is realized as si- or shi- depending upon whether the stem contains a post-alveolar sibilant. For example, while sido ('it is hot' perfective) has the first form, shibeezh ('it is boiled' perfective), the stem-final /ʒ/ triggers the change to /ʃ/.

D-effect

A particular type of morphophonemic alternation (or mutation) in Athabascan languages called d-effect is found in Navajo. In most cases, the alternation is a fortition (or strengthening) process. The initial consonant of a verb stem alternates with a strengthened consonant when it is preceded by a /-t-/ (orthographic -d-) "classifier" prefix or the /-Vt-/ first person dual subject prefix. [14] The underlying /t/ of these prefixes is absorbed into the following stem. D-effect can be viewed prosodically as the result of a phonotactic constraint on consonant clusters that would otherwise result from the concatenation of underlying segments. [15] There is thus an interaction between a requirement for the grammatical information to be expressed in the surface form and an avoidance of having sequences of consonants (see the syllable section for more on phonotactics).

The fortition is typically a change from continuant to affricate or continuant to stop (i.e., adding a period of closure to the articulation). However, other changes involve glottalization of the initial consonant:

Prefix consonant + Stem-initial consonant [16] Surface consonant Example
t- + -Z -ts /tʃʼéná-t-Zìt/[tʃʼéná-tsìt] chʼénádzid ('he woke up')
/t- + -L /-tl /ʔánéìnì-t-Laː/[ʔánéìnì-tlaː] ánéinidlaa ('you repaired it')
/t- + /-tʃ /ʔákʼídíní-t-Ʒéːʔ/[ʔákʼítíní-tʃéːʔ] ákʼídíníjééʼ ('you spit on yourself')
/t- + -j /-ts /nìː-Vt-jòɬ/[nìː-tsòɬ] niidzoł ('we two are driving them along')
(cf. /jìnòː-jòɬ/ yinooyoł 'he is driving them along')
/t- + /-k /jì-Vt-Ɣòʒ/[jìː-kòʒ] yiigozh ('we two are ticklish')
(cf. /jì-ɣòʒ/ yiwozh 'he is ticklish')
/t- + /-tʼ /nànìʃ-t-ʔìn/[nànìʃ-tʼìn] nanishtʼin ('I am hidden')
/t- + -m /-mʼ /jì-Vt-màs/[jìː-mʼàs] yiiʼmas ('we two are rolling along')
(cf. /jì-màs/ yimas 'he is rolling along')
/t- + -n /-nʼ /náːtòː-t-nìːt/[náːtòː-nʼìːt] náádooʼniid ('she said again')
/t- + -j /-jʼ /xònì-Vt-jóí/[xònìː-jʼóí] honiiʼyóí ('we two are energetic')
(cf. /xònìʃ-jʼóí/ honishyóí 'I am energetic')

The two occurrences of t- + -j in the chart above reflect two different patterns of d-effect involving stem-initial /j/. Often different underlying consonants are posited to explain the different alternation. The first alternation is posited as a result of underlying t- + leading to a d-effect mutation of /dz/. The other is t- + -j resulting in /jˀ/. (See the velar /ɣ/, palatal /j/ section for further explanation.)

Another example of d-effect influences not the stem-initial consonant but the classifier prefix. When the /-Vt-/ first person dual subject prefix precedes the /-ɬ-/ (orthographic -ł-) classifier prefix, the /-ɬ-/ classifier is realized as voiced [l]:

Prefix consonant + Classifier consonant Surface consonant Example
t- + -ɬ- -l- /jì-Vt-ɬ-Ʒõ̀ːh/[jìː-l-ʒõ̀ːh] yiilzhǫǫh ('we two tame it')

Other

  • n > high tone
  • expressive x clusters

Vowels

Navajo has four contrastive vowel qualities [i, e, o, ɑ] at three different vowel heights (high, mid, low) and a front-back contrast between the mid vowels [e, o]. There are also two contrastive vowel lengths and a contrast in nasalization. This results in 16 phonemic vowels, shown below.

There is a phonetic vowel quality difference between the long high vowel /iː/ (orthographic ii) and the short high vowel /i/ (orthographic i): the shorter vowel is significantly lower at [ ɪ] than its long counterpart. This phonetic difference is salient to native speakers, who will consider a short vowel at a higher position to be a mispronunciation. Similarly, short /e/ is pronounced [ ɛ]. Short /o/ is a bit more variable and more centralized, covering the space [ ɔ] ~ [ ɞ]. Notably, the variation in /o/ does not approach [ u], which is a true gap in the vowel space.

Although the nasalization is contrastive in the surface phonology, many instances of nasalized vowels can be derived from a sequence of Vowel + Nasal consonant in a more abstract analysis. Additionally, there are alternations between long and short vowels that are predictable.

There have been a number of somewhat different descriptions of Navajo vowels, which are conveniently summarized in McDonough (2003).

Acoustic phonetics

McDonough (2003) has acoustic measurements of the formants of Navajo long and short oral vowel pairs as pronounced by 10 female and 4 male native speakers. An earlier study ( McDonough, Ladefoged & George (1993)) has measurements from 7 female speakers.

Below are the median values of the first (F1) and second (F2) formants for these studies:

Tones

Navajo has two tones: high and low. Orthographically, high tone is marked with an acute accent ⟨á⟩ over the affected vowel, while low tone is left unmarked ⟨a⟩. This reflects the tonal polarity of Navajo, as syllables have low tone by default.

Long vowels normally have level tones ⟨áá, aa⟩. However, in grammatical contractions and in Spanish loan words such as béeso ('money' from Spanish peso), long vowels may have falling ⟨áa⟩ or rising ⟨aá⟩ tones.

The sonorant /n/ also carries tone when it is syllabic. Here again, the high tone is marked with an acute ⟨ń⟩ while the low tone is left unmarked ⟨n⟩.

Even though low tone is the default, these syllables are not underspecified for tone: they have a distinct phonetic tone, and their pitch is not merely a function of their environment. This contrasts with the related Carrier language. As in many languages, however, the pitches at the beginnings of Navajo vowels are lower after voiced consonants than after tenuis and aspirated consonants. After ejective consonants, only high tones are lowered, so that the distinction between high and low tone is reduced. However, the type of consonant has little effect on the pitch in the middle of the vowel, so that vowels have characteristic rising pitches after voiced consonants.

The pitch of a vowel is also affected by the tone of the previous syllable: in most cases, this has as great an effect on the pitch of a syllable as its own tone. However, this effect is effectively blocked by an intervening aspirated consonant. [17]

Tonological processes

Navajo nouns are simple: /kʰṍ/ [kʰṍ] kǫ́ ('fire'), /pi-tiɬ/ [pìtìɬ] bidił ('his blood'). Most long nouns are actually deverbal.

In verbs, with few exceptions, only stems may carry a high tone: C V(ː)(C)( T). Prefixes are mostly single consonants, C-, and do not carry tone. The one exception is the high-tone vocalic prefix /ʌ́n/-.[ clarification needed] Most other tone-bearing units in the Navajo verb are second stems or clitics.

All Navajo verbs can be analyzed as compounds, and this greatly simplifies the description of tone. There are two obligatory components, the "I" stem (for "inflection") and the "V" stem (for "verb"), each potentially bearing a high tone, and each preceded by its own prefixes. [18] In addition, the compound as a whole takes 'agreement' prefixes like the prefixes found on nouns. This entire word may then take proclitics, which may also carry tone:

clitics= agreement– (prefixes– I-stem) + (prefixes– V-stem)
tone tone tone

(Hyphens "–" mark prefixes, double hyphens "=" mark clitics, and plus signs "+" join compounds.)

Any high tones on clitics spread to the next syllable of the word only if it is short and located immediately before the verbal stem. This can be seen with the iterative clitic /ná/꞊. Compare

ha=ni-sh+ł-chaad
hanishchaad
'I card it (wool)'

and

ha=ná=ni-sh+ł-chaʼ
hanáníshchaʼ
'I usually card it (wool)'

where the clitic ná= creates a high tone on the following short pre-stem syllable in bold, but,

ha=ná=ni-iid+ł-chaʼ
hanániichaʼ
'we usually card it (wool)'

and

ha=ná=da=ni-oh+ł-chaʼ
hanídanołchaʼ
'you (pl) usually card it (wool)'

where it does not.

  • conjunct prefixes in verb stems are unmarked for tone (with a few exceptions) – they assimilate to the tones of neighboring prefixes
  • tones in disjunct prefixes and stems are underlying specified
  • certain enclitics (like the subordinator =go) affect the tones of preceding verb stems

Syllable

Stems. The stems (e.g. noun stems, verb stems, etc.) have the following syllable types:

C V(ː)( T)(C)

That is, all syllables must have a consonant onset C, a vowel nucleus V. The syllable may carry a high tone T, the vowel nucleus may be short or long, and there may optionally be a consonant coda.

Prefixes. Prefixes typically have a syllable structure of CV-, such as chʼí- ('out horizontally'). Exceptions to this are certain verbal prefixes, such as the classifiers (-ł-, -l-, -d-) that occur directly before the verb stem, which consist of a single consonant -C-. A few other verbal prefixes, such as naa- ('around') on the outer left edge of the verb have long vowels, CVV-. A few prefixes have more complex syllable shapes, such as hashtʼe- ('ready') (CVCCV-). Prefixes do not carry tone.

Some analyses, such as that of Harry Hoijer, consider conjunct verbal prefixes to have the syllable shape CV-. In other generative analyses, [19] the same prefixes are considered to have only underlying consonants of the shape C-. Then, in certain environments, an epenthetic vowel (the default vowel is ⟨i⟩) is inserted after the consonantal prefix.

Peg elements, segment insertion

All verbs must be at least disyllabic. Some verbs may only have a single overt nonsyllabic consonantal prefix, a prefix lacking an onset, or no prefix at all before the verb stem. Since all verbs are required to have two syllables, a meaningless prefix must be added to the verb to fulfill the disyllabic requirement. This prosodic prefix is known as a peg element in Athabascan terminology ( Edward Sapir used the term pepet vowel). For example, the verb meaning "she/he/they is/are crying" has the following morphological composition: Ø-Ø-cha, where both the imperfective modal prefix and the third-person subject prefix are phonologically null morphemes and the verb stem is -cha. In order for this verb to be complete, a yi- peg element must be prefixed to the verb stem, resulting in the verb form yicha. Other examples are the verb yishcha ('I am crying'), which is morphologically Ø-sh-cha (Ø- null imperfective modal, -sh- first-person singular subject, -cha verb stem) and wohcha ('you [2+] are crying'), which is Ø-oh-cha (Ø- null imperfective modal, -oh- second-person dual-plural subject, -cha verb stem). The glide consonant of the peg element is ⟨y⟩ before ⟨i⟩, ⟨w⟩ before ⟨o⟩, and ⟨gh⟩ before ⟨a⟩.

Notes

  1. ^ McDonough & Ladefoged (1993), p. ?.
  2. ^ McDonough (2003) and McDonough & Ladefoged (1993) find these to be affricates and not clusters. The acoustic difference between an affricate and a stop + fricative consonant cluster is the rate of increase in the amplitude of the frication noise (i.e. the rise time); affricates have a short rise time, consonant clusters have a longer rise time between the stop and fricative Johnson (2003:144–45).
  3. ^ Reichard (1945) reports that this variation is salient to Navajo speakers and that speakers with aspirated stops called the speakers with velar frication x da'ání ('⟨x⟩-speakers').
  4. ^ The velar frication on /tʰ, kʰ/ is also seen in the closely related Chiricahua Apache language; however, the Western Apache language does not typically have this velar aspiration. Hoijer (1942:?)
  5. ^ McDonough (2003), p. 130.
  6. ^ For example, Hoijer (1945a)
  7. ^ Similar observations have been noted for the closely related Western Apache language.
  8. ^ Hoijer (1945a) does not note the approximant-like nature of /ɣ/ before /a/ although he does do so for the labialized allophone.
  9. ^ Navajo verb stems have complex vowel ablaut involving different vowel qualities, length, nasalization, and tone. The verb stem "make bubbling noise" has the following verb stem-forms: -gháásh, -ghaash, -gháázh, -ghaazh, -ghash, -wosh.
  10. ^ Conventionally, these are written as voiced in Young & Morgan (1987).
  11. ^ These underlying representations are not fully segmented; since the focus is on the voicing of the stem-initial, only the prefix immediately preceding the stem is isolated from the rest of the prefix complex.
  12. ^ A third person subject is indicated in Navajo verbs by the absence of a subject prefix. A null set symbol Ø is usually used as a place holder (i.e. a zero morpheme) representing the third person subject. The last verb [jɪ̀ɰ̝ʷòʒ] yiwozh underlyingly consists of a single stem with no prefixes (or, alternately, a single zero morpheme prefix). However, there is a constraint on Navajo verbs in that they must contain at least two syllables and start with a consonant. In order to fulfill this requirement, a semantically empty prefix [jɪ-] yi- (called a peg prefix) must occur before the stem, thus creating an intervocalic environment which triggers the voicing assimilation. See the syllable section for further details.
  13. ^ An additional complication in this set of data is that the /ʃ-/ prefix is deleted before sibilant consonants /s, ʃ/.
  14. ^ The -d- classifier occurs in position 9 of the prefix template found in Young & Morgan (1987) while the -Vd- prefix occurs in position 8. See the classifier section below for more about classifier prefixes.
  15. ^ McDonough (2003), p. 60.
  16. ^ The continuant stem-initial consonants are represented here as archiphonemes /Z, L, Ʒ, Ɣ/ instead of voiced (or voiceless) continuants. See the voicing assimilation section for further details.
  17. ^ deJong & McDonough (1993).
  18. ^ These are actually roots, and the prefix-root unit is technically the stem, but the use of the word 'stem' for 'root' is widespread in Athabaskan studies.
  19. ^ For example, Wright (1983), Speas (1985), Speas (1990), McDonough (2003)

Bibliography

  • Hoijer, Harry (1942), "Phonetic and phonemic change in the Athapaskan languages", Language, 18 (3): 218–20, doi: 10.2307/409555, JSTOR  409555
  • Hoijer, Harry (1945a), Navaho phonology, University of New Mexico publications in anthropology (No. 1), Albuquerque: The University of New Mexico Press
  • Johnson, Keith (2003), Acoustic and Auditory Phonetics (Second ed.), Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing
  • McDonough, Joyce (2003), The Navajo Sound System, Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, ISBN  1-4020-1351-5
  • McDonough, Joyce; Ladefoged, Peter (1993), "Navajo stops", UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics, 84: 151–64
  • McDonough, Joyce; Ladefoged, Peter; George, H. (1993), "Navajo vowels and universal phonetic tendencies", University of California Working Papers in Phonetics, 84: 143–50
  • Reichard, Gladys A. (1945), "Linguistic diversity among the Navaho Indians", International Journal of American Linguistics, 11 (3): 156–68, doi: 10.1086/463866, S2CID  143774525
  • Sapir, Edward; Hoijer, Harry (1967), Phonology and morphology of the Navaho language, Berkeley: University of California Press.
  • Speas, Margaret (1990), Phrase Structure in Natural Language, Dordrect: Kluwer Academic Publishers
  • Speas, Margaret (1985), "Navajo Prefixes and Word Structure Typology", MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, 6, Cambridge: 86–111
  • Wright, Martha (1983), "The CV Skeleton and Verb Prefix Phonology in Navajo", Proceedings of NELS 14, Amherst{{ citation}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher ( link)
  • Young, Robert W.; Morgan, William Sr. (1987), The Navajo language: A grammar and colloquial dictionary (rev. ed.)., Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press, ISBN  0-8263-1014-1

Further reading

  • Cook, Eung-Do; Rice, Keren (1989). "Introduction". In Cook, Eung-Do; Rice, Keren (eds.). Athapaskan linguistics: Current perspectives on a language family. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 1–61. doi: 10.1515/9783110852394-002. ISBN  9783110852394.
  • Haile, Berard. (1941–1948). Learning Navaho, (Vols. 1–4). St. Michaels, AZ: St. Michael's Mission.
  • Hale, Kenneth. (1970–1972). Navajo linguistics (Nos. 1–4). (Unpublished manuscript). (Available online: www.swarthmore.edu/SocSci/tfernal1/nla/halearch/halearch.htm).
  • Hardy, Frank Wilton (1979). Navajo aspectual verb stem variation (PhD thesis). University of New Mexico.
  • Harris, Zellig S. (1945). "Navaho phonology and Hoijer's analysis". International Journal of American Linguistics. 11 (4): 239–246. doi: 10.1086/463877. S2CID  224806942.
  • Hoijer, Harry (1938). "The southern Athapaskan languages". American Anthropologist. 40 (1): 75–87. doi: 10.1525/aa.1938.40.1.02a00080.
  • Hoijer, Harry (1943). "Pitch accent in the Apachean languages". Language. 19 (1): 38–41. doi: 10.2307/410317. JSTOR  410317.
  • Hoijer, Harry (1945b). "Review of The Story of the Navajo Hail Chant by Gladys A. Reichard". International Journal of American Linguistics. 11 (2): 123–125. doi: 10.1086/463862.
  • Hoijer, Harry (1953). "Review of Navaho Grammar by Gladys A. Reichard". International Journal of American Linguistics. 19 (1): 78–83. doi: 10.1086/464194.
  • Hoijer, Harry (1970). A Navajo lexicon. University of California Publications in Linguistics. Vol. 78. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN  9780520094871.
  • Kari, James (1975). "The disjunct boundary in the Navajo and Tanaina verb prefix complexes". International Journal of American Linguistics. 41 (4): 330–345. doi: 10.1086/465374. S2CID  144924113.
  • Kari, James (1976). Navajo verb prefix phonology. Garland Publishing Co.
  • Krauss, Michael E. (1970). "Review of The Phonology and Morphology of the Navaho Language by Edward Sapir & Harry Hoijer". International Journal of American Linguistics. 36 (3): 220–228. doi: 10.1086/465115.
  • Krauss, Michael E.; Leer, Jeff (1981). Athabaskan, Eyak, and Tlingit sonorants. Alaska Native Language Center research papers. Vol. 5. Fairbanks, AK: University of Alaska, Alaska Native Language Center. ISBN  0-933769-35-0.
  • Ladefoged, Peter; Maddieson, Ian (1996). The sounds of the world's languages. Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Leer, Jeff (1979). Proto-Athabaskan verb stem variation I: Phonology. Alaska Native Language Center research papers. Vol. 1. Fairbanks, AK: University of Alaska, Alaska Native Language Center.
  • McDonough, Joyce (1990). Topics in the morphology and phonology of Navajo verbs (PhD thesis). University of Massachusetts Amherst.
  • McDonough, Joyce (1996). "Epenthesis in Navajo". In Jelinek, E.; Midgette, S.; Saxon, L.; Rice, K. (eds.). Athabaskan language studies: Essays in honor of Robert W. Young. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press. pp. 235–257.
  • McDonough, Joyce (1999). "Tone in Navajo". Anthropological Linguistics. 41 (4): 503–539. JSTOR  30028725.
  • McDonough, Joyce; Austin-Garrison, Martha (August 1994). "Vowel enhancement and dispersion in the vowel space of Western Navajo: a study of traditional Navajo speakers". UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics. 87: 93–104.
  • McDonough, Joyce; Ladefoged, Peter (1996). "The specification of stop contrasts in Navajo". In Nespor, M.; Vogel, I. (eds.). Dam phonology. HIL phonology papers. Vol. II. Holland Institute of Linguistics Publications. pp. 123–142.
  • Platero, Paul R. (1986). Diné bizaad bee naadzo: A conversational Navajo text for secondary schools, colleges and adults. Farmington, NM: Navajo Preparatory School.
  • Platero, Paul R.; Legah, Lorene; Platero, Linda S. (1985). Diné bizaad bee naʼadzo: A Navajo language literacy and grammar text. Farmington, NM: Navajo Language Institute.{{ cite book}}: CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list ( link)
  • Reichard, Gladys A. (1947). "Reply to Hoijer's review of The story of the Navajo hail chant". International Journal of American Linguistics. 13 (3): 193–196. doi: 10.1086/463951. S2CID  143545287.
  • Reichard, Gladys A. (1948). "Significance of aspiration in Navaho". International Journal of American Linguistics. 14 (1): 15–19. doi: 10.1086/463972. S2CID  144737470.
  • Reichard, Gladys A. (1951). Navaho grammar. Publications of the American Ethnological Society. Vol. 21. New York: J. J. Augustin. hdl: 2027/miun.acr7453.0021.001.
  • Rice, Keren (Fall 1997). "A reexamination of Proto-Athabaskan *y". Anthropological Linguistics. 39 (3): 423–436. JSTOR  30028997.
  • Sapir, Edward; Hoijer, Harry (1942). Navaho texts. William Dwight Whitney series. Linguistic Society of America. doi: 10.2307/j.ctt1x76d5g. JSTOR  j.ctt1x76d5g.
  • Saville, Muriel (1968). Navajo morphophonemics (PhD thesis). University of Texas.
  • Stanley, Richard (1969). The phonology of the Navajo verb (PhD thesis). MIT.
  • Witherspoon, Gary (1985). Diné Bizaad Bóhooʼaah for secondary schools, colleges, and adults. Farmington, NM: Navajo Language Institute.
  • Witherspoon, Gary (1986). Diné Bizaad Bóhooʼaah I: A conversational Navajo text for secondary schools, colleges and adults. Farmington, NM: Navajo Language Institute.